An official website of the United States government
Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.
Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.
- Publications
- Account settings
- Advanced Search
- Journal List
More on the MORE Life Experience Model: What We Have Learned (So Far)
Judith glück, susan bluck, nic m weststrate.
- Author information
- Article notes
- Copyright and License information
Corresponding author.
Issue date 2019.
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
We have all had difficult times and challenges in our lives, and most of us feel that we learned something from those experiences. At the same time, few people actually become wise in the course of their lives – while most of us become (or remain) well-adapted and happy, generally satisfied, or even bitter or depressed. Why is it that some people, but not others, grow wise over time by learning from life’s challenges (Linley & Joseph, 2004 )? In the MORE Life Experience Model (Glück & Bluck, 2013 ), we argued that life challenges are catalysts for the development of wisdom, and that psychological resources crucially influence how people appraise life challenges, how they deal with them, and how they integrate them into their life story as time goes on. Based on the literature on wisdom and growth from challenging experiences, we proposed five resources as important for the development of wisdom: Mastery, Openness, Reflectivity, and Emotion Regulation including Empathy – in short, MORE.
Since proposing the model, we have conducted a first empirical test of its predictions. This paper describes our expected and unexpected findings, which provide insights that we integrate to further refine and elaborate the MORE Life Experience Model. First, we describe the theoretical and empirical background of the original model.
The MORE Life Experience Model
Life challenges as catalysts of wisdom.
Our first proposition was that life experiences that challenge a person’s beliefs and worldviews are: (a) the main life context in which wisdom manifests, and (b) necessary for the continual development of wisdom. When people are asked about situations in their life in which they did something wise, most describe an important, difficult situation such as a long-term life decision, a complicated social conflict, or a serious illness (Glück, Bluck, Baron, & McAdams, 2005 ). That is, wisdom is both manifest, and has a chance to further grow, when individuals face difficult obstacles or decisions that force them to question their own priorities and worldviews. We proposed specifically that use of the MORE resources should help individuals to manifest wisdom in the face of these challenging situations.
We also suggested that use of the MORE resources in reflecting on life challenges enables people to grow wiser. Life experience is agreed upon by laypeople and wisdom researchers (e.g., Ardelt, 2005 ; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000 ; Bluck & Glück, 2005 ; Glück & Bluck, 2011 ; Jeste, Ardelt, Blazer, Kraemer, Vaillant, & Meeks, 2010 ; Sternberg, 1998 ; Webster, 2003 ) as playing an important role in the continued development of wisdom. The MORE Life Experience Model draws specifically upon work suggesting that crises and obstacles can challenge people’s worldviews and thereby broaden their perspective (see also Ardelt; 2005 ; Kinnier, Tribbensee, Rose, & Vaughan, 2001 ; Kramer, 2003 ) and that particular psychological resources are essential for growth in the wake of adversity (e.g., Aldwin, 2007 ; Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006 ; Joseph & Linley, 2005 ).
The MORE Resources
We identified five resources that were repeatedly mentioned in the literature on life-span development, the development of wisdom, and growth from adversity, and are conceptually linked to growth in wisdom from difficult life experiences.
Sense of mastery . Most people have a healthy sense of illusory control that helps maintain stability and well-being (e.g., Peterson & Bossio, 2001 ; Taylor & Brown, 1988 ). Wise individuals, however, are more realistically aware of the uncertainty and unpredictability of life (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000 ; McKee & Barber, 1999 ) while also feeling that, having learned from experience, they will somehow be able to master whatever happens. Thus, mastery is a dialectical concept that combines full awareness of life’s uncontrollability and unpredictability with trust in one’s own ability to cope. Wise individuals are able to take action on things that they can control and accept things that they cannot control (Ardelt, 2005 ).
Openness . Wise individuals are interested in viewing situations from multiple perspectives (e.g., Staudinger, Lopez, & Baltes, 1997 ; Webster, 2003 ). They are non-judgmental, accept goals and values that differ from their own, and enjoy learning from others. They seek out new experiences and adapt well to the changes life inevitably brings. Webster ( 2003 , 2007 ) considers openness as one of five components of wisdom itself. We propose, however, that openness is a precursor to wisdom (cf. Ardelt, 2011 ) because it enables people to learn from experiences and from others. Openness is certainly also a component of wisdom, and it is an interesting question in itself how wise individuals are able to maintain their openness to new ideas and experiences way into old age, where openness usually declines (Glück, in press). However, we believe that it is there before wisdom is – it paves the way for new experiences and new perspectives that become part of an individual’s wisdom-related knowledge and expertise.
Reflectivity . We define reflectivity as a person’s motivation to think about complex issues in a complex way. Reflective people look back on life experiences and think deeply about them. They are willing to question their own past and current views and behavior, as their goal is to develop a deeper understanding and not to reassure their own views. Reflection is a key ingredient of wisdom in laypeople’s conceptions (Bluck & Glück, 2005 ) and a component of Ardelt’s ( 2003 , 2004 ) and Webster’s ( 2003 , 2007 ) definitions of wisdom. We assume that being reflective, like being open, sets the stage for the development and growth of wisdom (Staudinger, 2001 ).
Emotion regulation. Wise individuals are attentive to their emotions, tolerant of ambivalent feelings, and able to manage emotion as fits the situation. Laypeople’s theories of wisdom often include calm in the face of conflict, which is arguably the most visible sign of emotion regulation (Bluck & Glück, 2005 ), and of particular importance when dealing with negative events (Troy, Wilhelm, Shallcross, & Mauss, 2010 ). As their aim is to understand life more fully, wise individuals neither suppress negative feelings nor dwell on them extensively (Kunzmann, 2004 ). They are also able to appreciate the positive things in life (König & Glück, 2014 ). As with openness, Webster’s model of wisdom (Webster, 2003 , 2007 ) includes emotion regulation as a component, while we and Ardelt ( 2011 ) view it as a developmental precursor.
Empathy. We view empathy as an important precondition for the development of wisdom: those able to take others’ perspectives are more likely to develop a view of life that takes the needs of others and the common good into account (Jeste et al., 2010 ; Sternberg, 1998 ). Ardelt ( 2003 ) proposed compassion as the core of the affective dimension of wisdom. Such concern for others is also a component of wisdom in lay theories (Bluck & Glück, 2005 ): For wise individuals, concern for others is not limited to family or friends but includes a larger view of those in need of support, across humanity (Levenson, Jennings, Aldwin, & Shiraishi, 2005 ). Individuals popularly cited as wise have often created significant positive change in the world (Weststrate, Ferrari, & Ardelt, 2016 ).
Testing the MORE Life Experience Model: the First Empirical Study
The MORE Life Experience Model argues that people higher in a sense of mastery, openness, reflectivity, emotion regulation, and empathy are more likely than others both to display wisdom in dealing with life challenges and to continue growing towards greater wisdom across a lifetime. A strict test of these developmental dynamics would require longitudinal data of individuals before, during and after naturally-occurring stressful life events. Given the difficulty and duration of that type of research, as a first step, we empirically tested the hypothesis that the MORE resources are statistically correlated with measures of wisdom in a cross-sectional design. In the following, we describe this study and its main findings.
Participants. The fact that wisdom is a rare phenomenon makes it somewhat hard to investigate – as we have learned, recruiting a general-population sample yields few highly wise individuals. Therefore, we tried to increase the proportion of wise individuals in our sample by using a wisdom nomination approach. Through newspapers and radio programs, community members were invited to nominate any person that they felt was particularly wise. Excluding self-nominations, 82 people were nominated, and 47 of them agreed to participate. For comparison purposes, 123 other participants were recruited through invitation letters sent to a large population sample in the same region. Thus, the final sample consisted of 170 participants (90 women, 80 men), most of whom (86.5%) were 40 – 92 years of age. All participants came to the lab for two interview sessions and filled out some questionnaires at home (details are described in Glück et al., 2013 ). They were paid € 70 (about U.S. $80.00) for participation.
Measures. How wisdom is measured can affect results significantly. Most existing measures of wisdom are either self-report scales or open-ended responses to life problems. The two methods are not highly correlated (Glück, 2018 ; Glück et al., 2013 ). Self-report measures present a paradox: people who describe themselves as wise may lack the self-reflectivity that defines wisdom (Aldwin, 2009 ; Glück et al., 2013 ). Open-ended measures are not subject to such biases, but highly effort-consuming to analyze, and may overemphasize intellectual aspects of wisdom (Ardelt, 2004 ). We thus measured wisdom and the MORE resources using self-report measures in a relatively large sample and open-ended measures in a subsample, to ensure our results would not be an artifact of the methods used. The subsample consisted of the 47 wisdom nominees and 47 control participants parallel to the nominees in age and gender (for details, see Glück et al., 2013 ).
Participants completed three self-report measures of wisdom . The Self-Assessed Wisdom Scale (SAWS; Webster, 2007 ) measures five components of wisdom: openness, emotional regulation, humor, critical life experience, and reminiscence and reflectiveness. The Three-Dimensional Wisdom Scale (3D-WS; Ardelt, 2003 ) assesses cognitive, reflective, and compassionate dimensions of wisdom. The revised Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI; Levenson et al., 2005 ) defines wisdom as self-transcendence. Reliabilities were satisfactory to excellent for all self-report measures (see Glück et al., 2013 ).
The open-ended wisdom measure came from the Berlin Wisdom Paradigm (BWP; Staudinger, Smith, & Baltes, 1994 ). After some practice with thinking aloud, participants were presented with a standard life-review problem: “In reflecting over their life, people sometimes realize that they have not achieved what they had once wanted to achieve. What could a person consider and do in such a situation?” Response transcripts were evaluated by trained raters following the BWP manual (Staudinger et al., 1994 ). There were two independent raters for each criterion: factual knowledge, procedural knowledge, life-span contextualism, value relativism, and recognition/management of uncertainty.
As self-report measures of the MORE resources , we used scales or relevant parts of scales from well-established measures. Sense of Mastery includes eight items from Wagnild and Young’s ( 1993 ) Resilience Scale referring to acceptance of uncontrollability (e.g., “I do not dwell on things that I can’t do anything about”) and dealing with hardship (“I can get through difficult times because I’ve experienced difficulty before”); Cronbach’s α was .76. Openness was assessed with the Openness scale of the NEO FFI (12 items, α = .78). For Reflectivity, five items were selected from the Psychological Mindedness Scale (Conte, Plutchik, Jung, Picard, Karasu, & Lotterman, 1990 ; e.g., “I often find myself thinking about what made me act in a certain way”) and the directive-function scale of the Talking About Life Experiences scale (Bluck & Alea, 2002 ; e.g., “I think back over my life when I want to learn from my past mistakes”; α = .77). Emotion regulation was measured using the subscales for perception (9 items, e.g., “I am often uncertain about what I am feeling”) and regulation (6 items, e.g., “When I am afraid of something, there is little I can do about it”) of one’s own emotions from the German-language Emotional Competence Questionnaire (Freudenthaler & Neubauer, 2005 ; α = .83). Empathy was assessed by the Empathic Concern subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1983 ; α = .68).
Our open-ended measure of the MORE resources was an interview about a difficult interpersonal conflict from the participants’ past. As explained earlier, we propose that the MORE resources influence how people reflect upon past life challenges. Therefore, we assumed that participants’ levels of the MORE resources should manifest themselves in the way they talked about such experiences. Their accounts of a conflict should enable us to evaluate their willingness and ability to question their own position and take the opponent’s perspective in retrospect. In the interview, they were first asked to make a list of serious interpersonal conflicts they had encountered. Then, they selected the most difficult conflict they wanted to talk about. They were asked to freely narrate the event and then answer questions concerning what had happened later, how they and their opponent had felt at the time, how they felt about the experience now, and whether they had learned something from it. The interview transcripts were rated by two trained student raters for each MORE resource using 4-point scales from 0 = “no indication of the resource” to 3 = “extraordinary level of the resource”. Table 1 describes the rating criteria for each MORE resource and illustrates them with quotations from the interviews. Participants were also interviewed about another difficult event from their past, but those interviews were difficult to rate in a reliable way. Therefore, we focus on the conflict interviews in the following.
Rating Criteria and Sample Quotations for the MORE Resources
Note: Resources were rated on scales from 0 (“no indication of the resource”) to 3 (“high level of the resource”). Each resource included two aspects, and level 3 was only coded if both aspects were present in the narrative.
Table 2 shows inter-rater reliabilities for the BWP criteria and the MORE resources. Inter-rater reliabilities for the BWP were comparable to other studies (Glück & Baltes, 2006 ; Mickler & Staudinger, 2008 ); the total BWP score had a Cronbach’s α of .85. Reliabilities for the conflict interview were in the same range, with a Cronbach’s α of .88.
Inter-Rater Reliabilities for the MORE Resource and BWP Criterion Ratings
We used structural equation models to test our hypothesis that the MORE resources would be significantly correlated to wisdom. Separate analyses were performed for the self-report and the open-ended measures. In both cases, we first fit separate measurement models for the resources and the wisdom measures and then combined the optimized measurement models to test the full structural model.
Self-report measures. Table 3 displays the correlations between the self-report measures of the MORE resources and wisdom. Interestingly, there was some differentiation among the MORE resources: there were substantial correlations between mastery and emotion regulation and between empathy and reflectivity, but not across these two groups except for openness, which had significant correlations with both emotion regulation and empathy. Consistent with that, a one-factor measurement model for the self-report MORE measures did not fit the data. A two-factor model with mastery and emotion regulation on one factor, reflectivity and empathy on the other, and openness loading on both factors fit the data well. The correlations among the three wisdom scales were all significant (Glück et al., 2013 ), and they loaded on one factor.
Correlations between Self-report Measures of the MORE Resources and Wisdom (N = 150)
Using these measurement models, we next fitted a structural model predicting the wisdom factor from the two MORE factors. As Table 3 shows, the correlations between the MORE resources and the wisdom scales were all significant except for a zero correlation between reflectivity and the 3D-WS. However, the model did not fit the data well, and the fit remained unsatisfactory after a residual negative correlation between the 3D-WS and SAWS, suggested by modification indices, was permitted, χ 2 (19) = 80.275, p < .001, GFI = .870, CFI = .839, RMSEA = .164. The remaining modification indices all required correlations between specific MORE resources and wisdom scales, which would have run against the logic of the model. Therefore, the structural model was retained as it was (see Figure 1 ). In spite of the problems with model fit, both resource factors were strong predictors of wisdom with standardized regression weights of .76 and .69. 1
Structural equation models for the self-report measures of the MORE resources and wisdom. Coefficients are standardized estimates
Open-ended measures. Table 4 displays the correlations between the MORE resource ratings of the conflict interviews and the BWP criteria. Here, the measurement model for the MORE resources did not require differentiation into two factors: the one-factor model fit the data very well, and the same was true for the five Berlin criteria. The structural model showed highly satisfactory fit, χ 2 (34) = 38.324, p = .280, GFI = .915, CFI = .986, RMSEA = .040. It is displayed in Figure 2 . As the figure shows, all paths were significant and substantial. The MORE resource ratings from the conflict interview predicted performance criteria in the Berlin wisdom paradigm with a significant regression weight of .45.
Correlations between Interview Ratings of the MORE Resources and Criteria of the Berlin Wisdom Paradigm (N = 82)
Structural equation models for the open-ended measures of the MORE resources and wisdom. Coefficients are standardized estimates
This study provided a first, cross-sectional test of the hypothesis that the MORE resources are related to wisdom across different measurement methods. The models for both self-report and open-ended narrative measures showed significant and substantial positive relationships between the MORE resources and wisdom. This is not particularly surprising with respect to the self-report data: in our experience, self-report measures of positively valued constructs have a general tendency to covary, if only because they tap into people’s general way of thinking about themselves (Glück, 2018 ; Glück et al., 2013 ). Therefore, we were a bit surprised to find the clear dissociation between emotion regulation and mastery on the one hand and empathy and reflectivity on the other. There are at least two explanations for this finding. First, one could argue that emotion regulation and a sense of mastery are about a person’s “internal” way of dealing with experiences, by controlling emotions and by deciding whether to take action or to accept a given situation. Empathy and reflectivity are about taking “external” perspectives: imagining how another person is feeling and thinking about oneself from a self-distanced perspective. On the other hand, the two types of resources may have different developmental timelines: empathy and reflectivity may be predecessors of wisdom, whereas emotion regulation and mastery are acquired with experience and may co-develop with wisdom. We will come back to this point later.
Interestingly, no such dissociation was found for the open-ended data: in the autobiographical interviews about a difficult conflict, participants’ levels of all MORE resources were loading on one factor. Thus, the resources that people utilize when they are reflecting on a concrete event from their past are more closely related than people’s self-evaluations of those resources as traits. Perhaps this is the case because difficult conflicts require both perspective-taking and internal regulation capacities.
In sum, we consider our findings as an encouraging first step in our growing understanding of how wisdom develops. The next step will be to investigate how the resources longitudinally interact with life experiences: the MORE Life Experience Model predicts that the resources that individuals bring to a life challenge influence how they deal with the challenge, but also whether they then grow wiser in its aftermath.
Lessons We Have Learned
We have gained a number of insights from this study and some smaller “side studies” that we did in connection with it. We will detail these lessons in the following section. First, we discuss lessons that led us to refine the model, then, lessons that extend the model in new directions.
Optimizing the MORE Life Experience Model
Single events vs. life phases. Sometimes, participants had difficulty pinpointing one specific event that led to an important insight or a change in worldview. They might have been struggling with a problem for a long time and then at some point, a relatively minor experience, such as a conversation they had or a book that they read, gave them a whole new perspective on the issue. In addition, even when people felt that a single difficult event had led to a major insight, it was often only in the aftermath of the event, when they had the time and nerves to reflect upon what had happened, that they realized how much they had changed. Thus, we would like to shift the focus away from life-changing events to life-changing insights – which often but not always happen in the course of life-changing events. In future studies, we plan to ask participants more directly about the experiences that had the strongest effect on their views about life (see, e.g., Weststrate, Ferrari, Fournier, & McLean, 2018 ) instead of asking them about difficult events and lessons they derived from them.
Renaming some resources. First, while the acronym “MORE” was well suited to convey the general idea that wisdom should be related to “more life experience” as well as to M, O, R, and E/E plus life experience, we may not have chosen the optimal labels for all resources. We would like to change the labels of the “M” and “E” resources as follows.
Managing uncertainty and uncontrollability. The original label for the “M” resource, “sense of mastery,” is too closely related to concepts like self-efficacy and internal control beliefs. It captures the trust that wise individuals have in their own ability to master whatever may happen in their life, but it may not convey their above-average awareness of the unpredictability and uncontrollability of human life. We consider both sides to be equally important: wise individuals neither overestimate nor underestimate their control and knowledge about the world. As mentioned earlier, the psychological literature suggests that most people overestimate the control they have over their life and that these control illusions are actually beneficial to their well-being (Peterson & Bossio, 2001 ; Taylor & Brown, 1988 ). On the other hand, underestimating one’s control has been related to learned helplessness and associated with depression (Seligman, 1975 ); thus, there may also be people for whom wisdom comes from realizing that they actually do have control over important parts of their lives. However, as the majority of people tends to overestimate their control and other conceptions of wisdom have also included awareness of uncertainty as a criterion (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000 ; Grossmann, 2017 ), we prefer to emphasize in the new “M” that wise individuals are fully aware of the limitations of their control and knowledge, but able to manage this awareness well.
Emotional sensitivity and regulation. The original labels for the “E”, emotion regulation and empathy, were somewhat imprecise. Emotion regulation is often used for all phases of perceiving and managing one’s own feelings (e.g., Gross & Thompson, 2007 ). Empathy is usually distinguished from sympathy in that it refers to being aware of others’ emotions without necessarily sharing them. Our conception of the “E” resource is meant to convey that wisdom involves both an attentiveness and sensitivity to the feelings of oneself and others and the ability to regulate them, so as to remain (relatively) calm and to calm down others in challenging situations. Thus, the distinction between the two aspects of the “E” resource is no longer between the self and others but between (a) sensitivity to emotions, which involves being attentive to one’s own and others’ feelings and taking them seriously even if they are unwanted, and (b) regulation of one’s own and others’ emotions as a context requires, which includes the ability to maintain one’s calm even in emotionally challenging situations. As discussed later, sensitivity may actually be an early predecessor of wisdom like openness or empathy, whereas emotion regulation is more of an acquired competence that is learned with experience over the lifespan.
Differentiating reflectivity. One important step forward in our work has been specifying more precisely what kind of reflection about experiences can foster wisdom. Virtually every theory of wisdom involves some aspect of reflection or reflectivity, and virtually all authors agree that it is not having had certain experiences per se, but having reflected upon them that leads to wisdom (e.g., Ardelt, 2005 ; Glück & Bluck, 2013 ; Staudinger, 2001 ; Webster, 2007 ). However, what authors mean by reflection varies considerably. For example, Webster ( 2007 ) defined his subcomponent of reminiscence and reflectiveness as “seeking to understand and derive insight from both our mistakes and successes“ (p. 168) whereas Ardelt ( 2003 ) defined her reflective dimension as “looking at phenomena and events from many different perspectives to develop self-awareness and self-insight, [a practice that] will gradually reduce one’s self-centeredness, subjectivity, and projections, and increase one’s insight into the true nature of things, including the motivations of one’s own and other people’s behavior” (p. 278). These two conceptions touch upon somewhat different aspects of reflection; in fact, we found a low but significant negative correlation between them (Glück et al., 2013 ).
How do we understand reflectivity (originally called “reflective attitude”) in the MORE Life Experience Model? In a way, our conception combines Ardelt’s and Webster’s ideas: we believe that thinking back upon experiences is necessary but not sufficient for developing wisdom; how one thinks about them matters as well. In line with Ardelt’s characterization, wise individuals reflect upon experiences with the aim of gaining insights and learning more about themselves and life in general. Weststrate and Glück ( 2017 ) distinguished two forms of reflection in the interview transcripts from the study described earlier. Exploratory processing is an analytical and interpretive way of reflecting about life events that emphasizes meaning-making (i.e., extracting lessons and insights), complexity, and growth from the past. For example, a participant in our study who had a son born with severe mental disabilities said, “I learned trust and acceptance. I am still learning. I am learning the whole time. I very often say that my oldest son is my greatest teacher. […] I just realize that accompanying my son’s life, I am in a constant learning process… I think this has given me strength.” Redemptive processing , on the other hand, describes the tendency to transform an initially negative experience into an emotionally positive one, leading to sense of emotional closure and event resolution. A participant who had survived cancer said, “I have a very positive attitude. I thank my organs every day for working well. […] in retrospect, I am glad that I had cancer. […] Feelings of gratitude… I do not think about the cancer itself anymore. That is done. It is in the past. It doesn’t make sense to give in to the fear that it could come back.” (Weststrate & Glück, 2017 , p. 807)
Studies have shown that exploratory processing of negative experiences is related to psychological maturity, while redemptive processing is related to happiness and well-being (e.g., King, Scollon, Ramsey, & Williams, 2000 ; Lilgendahl & McAdams, 2011 ; Pals, 2006 ). In our data, exploratory processing was correlated with wisdom, whereas redemptive processing was associated with well-being (Weststrate & Glück, 2017 ). Thus, wisdom-fostering reflectivity is exploratory in focus, aimed at learning about life in its complexity, and not redemptive, aimed at achieving closure and feeling better.
Manifestational vs. developmental resources and their different developmental timelines. In our original model, we assumed that the same resources foster the manifestation of wisdom during life challenges and the development of wisdom from life experiences: a person higher in the MORE resources would deal with a difficult situation in a wiser way and would also be more likely to grow even wiser from that experience. We still believe in the first assumption: a person dealing wisely with a difficult situation will be able to manage uncertainty and uncontrollability, open to alternative views, reflective of his or her views and behaviors, and sensitive to his or her own emotions and those of others involved and able to regulate them as the situation requires. The idea that these same resources also foster the development of wisdom, however, needs some differentiation. Both theoretical considerations and the dissociation we found for the self-report measures suggest that the resources have different developmental trajectories and different ways of interacting with wisdom in the course of development. Some of the resources – especially openness and emotional sensitivity – may be relatively early predecessors of wisdom, already present to individually different degrees in children. Reflectivity is probably learned from experience, starting early on – parents and caregivers can be models of critical self-reflection (or of defensiveness and denial). On the other hand, learning to manage uncertainty and uncontrollability probably requires relevant life experiences. It is therefore most likely to develop as people move from the growth-oriented, self-confident, expansive mindset of adolescence and young adulthood towards a more balanced view of their own power and its limitations in middle adulthood, by which point most individuals have experienced many life challenges. Similarly, while most people learn the “basics” of emotion regulation in childhood, extraordinary levels of this resource may develop in the course of adulthood as people are faced with more difficult emotional challenges such as divorce or serious illness. There are certainly individual differences in the developmental trajectories of all the resources depending on people’s individual experiences. But generally, people at different ages are likely to show somewhat different constellations of the MORE resources.
Importantly, we also believe that the resources interact dynamically with one another. Ideally, they foster each other’s development over time (Glück & Bluck, 2013 ). For example, if a child who is highly empathetic and emotionally perceptive also has the cognitive resources and environmental modelling and support necessary to acquire reflectivity, he or she is likely to develop effective emotion-regulation skills and an awareness of the limitations of his or her control. These skills, in turn, may enable him or her to become an extraordinary source of support and advice for people in need without burning out emotionally in the process.
Optimal vs. maximal levels of the resources. We have also found that for all five MORE resources, the optimal level may not be the maximum possible. It is possible to be so aware of uncertainty and uncontrollability that one becomes helpless, so open to others’ views that one cannot hold one’s own positions, so self-critical that one loses any self-confidence, so sensitive to others’ feelings and concerned about acquiescing them that one sacrifices one’s own well-being. Thus, while for most people becoming wiser means gaining a bit more distance from themselves and learning to take others’ perspectives, for others it may mean building trust in their own feelings, standing by their own values, and taking care of their own needs as well as those of others. Wisdom is a matter of balance more than extremes, and it manifests itself in the way individuals deal with specific, contextualized problems where optimal solutions may not always be possible (Sternberg, 1998 ).
Extending the MORE Life Experience Model
Other potential resources. One question that we have repeatedly discussed is whether the original five MORE resources are really the most important possible ones. Even if, as we showed earlier, the relationships between them differ somewhat by method of assessment, they are related both empirically and conceptually. In fact, we believe that in their dynamic developmental interaction they form a kind of “self-reinforcing syndrome”. For example, openness and empathy are likely to reinforce a self-reflective attitude, and such an attitude is likely to foster the development of emotion regulation skills, which may again help people remain open to others’ perspectives. It is an interesting question whether this “wisdom syndrome,” which we imagine as a kind of general mindset, includes other attitudes and capacities as well, some of which may even be more specific to wisdom than the ones we have described.
In her master’s thesis, our project member Lara Dorner drew upon the literature on growth in psychological and psychotherapeutic contexts to identify other growth-fostering resources that would also seem relevant to wisdom (Christopher, 2004 ; Curnow, 1999 ; Deci & Ryan, 2008 ; Joseph & Linley, 2005 ; Jung, 1971 ; Kramer, 1990 ; Labouvie-Vief, 1990 ; Levenson, 2009 ; Linley, 2003 ; Pascual-Leone, 1990 ; Rathunde, 2010 ; Rogers, 1964 ; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004 ). She identified six resources that came up across various conceptions of growth (Dorner, 2012 ). The arguably most important ones are process orientation and self-integration. Process orientation is a view of life as continuous learning and growth. Process-oriented individuals know that change is inherent in life and that negative experiences are unavoidable; rather than avoiding these challenges and contradictions, they are open to them and embrace the insights they bring with them Self-integration is perceptiveness to and acceptance of one’s own emotions, intuitions, and physical sensations. Self-integrative individuals do not suppress or ignore these perceptions, but are attentive to and accepting of them, even if they run against their ideal of how they would like to be. They aim at integrating even complex and contradictory facets into their own self-concept, which leads to a continually more complex view of the self. Acceptance and trust is a general attitude toward life that is able to look at things and let them happen, trusting that things will be okay, or if they are not, one will be able to deal with them, instead of constantly needing to take action and control. Self-determination is a way of living one’s life that takes one’s own individual needs and personality into account, follows one’s intrinsic motivations, and does not care about external evaluations or reinforcements. Self-determined individuals take responsibility for their actions as they act in accordance with their own self, and value the autonomy and authenticity of others as well. Self-transcendence is a way of experiencing the world that is not centered on one’s own self. Self-transcendent individuals do not feel the need to evaluate others, do not feel threatened when others disagree with them or prove them wrong, and do not depend on the admiration of others. They are compassionate and unselfish as they feel deeply connected to others and the world at large.
These six resources have been shown to be characteristic of growth processes that happen as individuals grow from difficult experiences both inside and outside psychotherapeutic contexts. They have some obvious conceptual relations to the MORE resources (for example, self-integration is related to emotional sensitivity, and process orientation to managing uncertainty), and we find indications of them in our interview transcripts. Thus, they are likely to be facets of the “wisdom-resource syndrome” as well. We selected the original five MORE resources because they were relatively close to established psychological concepts, which made it easier to conceptualize and measure them. It is an open question for future conceptual and empirical work whether additional resources might be added to the model.
Situational variability of wisdom. One of our most important insights concerns the question whether wisdom is a stable personal trait or a more fluctuating phenomenon. The idea that people’s wisdom varies across situational contexts has been supported by experimental research (overview in Grossmann, 2017 ) as well as by studies showing that most people can recall situations in which they did something wise (Bluck & Glück, 2004 ; Glück et al., 2005 ). Our research suggests, however, that there is variation not just in how wisely people act across different situations, but also in how wisely they reflect upon past experiences. As described earlier, participants in our study were interviewed about two different experiences: a conflict and a difficult event. We did not only have raters who evaluated the interview transcripts for the MORE resources, but also raters for wisdom, as we thought that it might be possible to measure wisdom by interviewing participants about life challenges. Trained student raters evaluated each transcript concerning the components of three different wisdom models: the Three-Dimensional Wisdom Model (Ardelt, 2003 ), the Berlin wisdom paradigm (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000 ), and the Bremen wisdom paradigm (Mickler & Staudinger, 2008 ). A fourth group of so-called “lay raters” rated the transcripts for wisdom using their own understanding of wisdom. Interestingly, the correlations within each interview suggested that the different wisdom conceptions tap rather similar characteristics: the average correlation was .73 within the conflict interview and .69 within the difficult-event interview. However, the correlations across the interviews suggest much less commonality with an average of .31 (Glück, 2018 ). Thus, a participant might well have talked very wisely about a conflict from her past but much less wisely about the other difficult event and vice versa.
Much recent research has shown that wisdom varies by situations – the same person may act very wisely in one situation and much less wisely in another. In other words, wisdom is not only determined by a person’s stable personality, but also by situational context (Grossmann, 2017 ). Our findings suggest that wisdom varies even when the external context of life reflection is held constant: people who are talking about two different situations in the same interview room, with the same interviewer, may still be far wiser about one situation than about the other. Thus, how wisely we are able to think about a past experience varies as well. Different experiences have different meanings for us, they happened in different life phases and taught us different lessons. How wisely we think about them may also depend on how much we have thought about the event before, who we talked to about it, what kind of responses got from others as we talked about it. The stories we make of our past experiences are often constructed in close contact with others; thus, others may have a strong influence on how much wisdom we can gain from an experience. This insight, together with some others, has led us to think more generally about the role of interpersonal resources for wisdom.
The important role of interpersonal resources. We began to notice the importance of external resources early on in our study. In particular, Susanne König, a doctoral student and interviewer in the research project, noticed that wisdom nominees seemed to be talking about gratitude far more often than other participants did. Eventually she wrote her dissertation on the relationship between wisdom and gratitude, demonstrating that, indeed, wisdom nominees far more often mentioned spontaneously that they were grateful for something or someone (König & Glück, 2014 ). Asking the participants what they were most grateful for, she found four categories that were mentioned more often by wisdom nominees: life in general with all its ups and downs, their health, their faith, and their partners. Given that most of them were middle-aged and older adults who had been in their relationships for a long time, one would not necessarily expect them to still feel gratitude for having their partner. One participant described her relationship as the best “event” in her life: “… he feels it when I’m not feeling good, and I can talk to him about it, and yes, I am very grateful that I have such a wonderful relationship. I am happy and grateful that I have him.”
Beyond intimate relationships, we also saw the importance of other people for wise individuals in an ethnographic study that another project member, Katja Naschenweng, carried out (for other ethnographic work on wisdom see Edmondson, 2005 , 2013 ). She wanted to study the small tribe of wise individuals just as one would an indigenous people in a distant corner of the world: by observing how they live their lives. She did so with five particularly wise participants of our study. Among several interesting commonalities she found between those quite different people, one was that while they lived somewhat contemplative lives in quiet places, they had not at all turned away from the outside world: they used media actively and selectively, they were very interested in art, literature, and philosophy, and they valued their active social lives. They considered their partners, family, and friends as important sources of not just happiness, but also insight. One participant said, “You need people with whom you can discuss issues, not just the usual blah-blah. We talk about things that are really important to us. I grow through my friendships and relationships. Sometimes I really want to be challenged in those conversations” (see also Weststrate & Glück, 2017 ).
In sum, these findings drew our attention to the importance of external, especially interpersonal resources for wisdom. How much and with whom we talk about our experiences and what we make of them may be as important as our internal ways of reflecting upon them. There may be different ways of telling stories that parallel the different forms of reflection that we have identified. Sometimes we talk about an experience for redemption: we know exactly what we want the listener to say to make us feel better, and we choose our audience and tell the story so as to elicit that reaction. At other times, we want to explore, to get to see a viewpoint, to perhaps gain a new understanding. This form of storytelling is clearly more likely to lead to new insights. Perhaps wise people are less reluctant to explore their own experiences by talking about them in this exploratory way than most of us are. This idea may suggest a Vygotskyan perspective (e.g., Kozulin, 2014 ) on the development of wisdom. 2 Perhaps there is a “zone of proximal development” for wisdom in the sense that people’s previous life experiences and internal resources determine the extent to which they can grow towards wisdom if they get the right external feedback. A mentor, psychotherapist, or simply a wise friend may be able to open up new perspectives on an experience or situation that may not only help them to resolve a problem but also to grow wiser (Igarashi, Levenson, & Aldwin, 2018 ).
Conclusion: The MORE Life Experience Model 2.0
What have we learned so far? A lot, we believe. First of all, our general idea that wisdom develops through a dynamic interaction between experiences and resources has remained unchanged. We have refined some aspects, such as the labels of some resources or our understanding of reflectivity, and broadened our perspective in some ways by including longer life phases and external resources. Thinking about the MORE Life Experience Model has also helped us gain a better understanding of what wisdom itself may be. Some researchers have argued that wisdom is essentially a form of complex, deep and broad knowledge (Baltes & Staudinger, 2000 ; Sternberg, 1998 ). Others believe that wisdom is not knowledge but a personality type (Ardelt, 2003 ). Both these conceptualizations are rendered somewhat incomplete by recent findings that wisdom varies by situation (Grossmann, 2017 ). Thinking about our model, we have come to believe that wisdom is both: deep, personal, experience-based knowledge about life that is acquired through and goes along with a certain mindset: the willingness and ability to take a broad, non-self-centered perspective on life with the goal of understanding it in all its complexity. People who have this mindset are more likely than others to learn more about life and accumulate wisdom-related knowledge over time, and they are more often able to deal with difficult situations wisely. How we can foster this mindset in human beings may be one of the most crucial questions for humanity at this point.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by the Austrian Research Fund FWF (grant number P21011) and the University of Chicago’s Defining Wisdom Initiative (John Templeton Foundation). We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for very insightful and constructive comments. Open access funding provided by University of Klagenfurt.
A potential problem with this analysis was the high degree of conceptual overlap between the MORE Life Experience Model and the subscales of the 3D-WS and the SAWS. The 3D-WS includes a compassionate and a reflective dimension, and the SAWS includes subscales labeled openness, emotional regulation, and reminiscence and reflectiveness. Therefore, we also ran a model that only included the ASTI, which conceptualizes wisdom as self-transcendence and assesses aspects that are relatively distant from the MORE resources (see Koller, Levenson, & Glück, 2017 ). To have more than one indicator, separate ASTI scores were computed for even and uneven item numbers ( r = . 78, p < .001). This model had a far better fit than the first model, χ 2 (12) = 33.199, p = .001, GFI = .946, CFI = .933, RMSEA = .109. The standardized regression weights were .82 for the “crystallized” and .57 for the “fluid” MORE resources.
We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out!
- Aldwin CM. Stress, coping, and development: An integrative approach. 2. New York: Guilford; 2007. [ Google Scholar ]
- Aldwin CM. Gender and Wisdom: A Brief Overview. Research in Human Development. 2009;6:1–8. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ardelt M. Empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Research on Aging. 2003;25:275–324. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ardelt M. Wisdom as expert knowledge system: A critical review of a contemporary operationalizations of an ancient concept. Human Development. 2004;47:257–285. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ardelt M. How wise people cope with crises and obstacles in life. ReVision: A Journal of Consciousness and Transformation. 2005;28:7–19. [ Google Scholar ]
- Ardelt M. The measurement of wisdom: A commentary on Taylor, Bates, and Webster’s comparison of the SAWS and 3D-WS. Experimental Aging Research. 2011;37:241–255. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.554509. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Baltes PB, Staudinger UM. A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist. 2000;55:122–136. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.122. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Bluck S, Alea N. Exploring the functions of autobiographical memory: Why do I remember the autumn? In: Webster JD, Haight BK, editors. Critical advances in reminiscence: From theory to application. New York, NY: Springer; 2002. pp. 61–75. [ Google Scholar ]
- Bluck S, Glück J. Making things better and learning a lesson: Experiencing wisdom across the lifespan. Journal of Personality. 2004;72:543–572. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00272.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Bluck S, Glück J. From the inside out: People’s implicit theories of wisdom. In: Sternberg RJ, Jordan J, editors. A handbook of wisdom: Psychological perspectives. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 84–109. [ Google Scholar ]
- Calhoun LG, Tedeschi RG, editors. Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research & practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2006. [ Google Scholar ]
- Christopher M. A broader view of trauma: A biopsychosocial-evolutionary view of the role of the traumatic stress response in the emergence of pathology and/or growth. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:75–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2003.12.003. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Conte HR, Plutchik R, Jung BB, Picard S, Karasu TB, Lotterman A. Psychological mindedness as a predictor of psychotherapy outcome: A preliminary report. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1990;31:426–431. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(90)90027-p. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Curnow T. Wisdom, intuition and ethics. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate Publishing; 1999. [ Google Scholar ]
- Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1983;44:113–126. [ Google Scholar ]
- Deci EL, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory: A macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne. 2008;49:182–185. [ Google Scholar ]
- Dorner, L. 2012. Deduktion von Wachstumsressourcen aus psychotherapeutisch-orientierten Theorien und ihre Integration in die Forschung zur Entwicklung von Weisheit. [Deduction of growth resources from psychotherapeutically oriented theories and their integration into research on the development of wisdom.] Unpublished diploma thesis, University of Klagenfurt, Austria.
- Edmondson R. Wisdom in later life: Ethnographic approaches. Ageing & Society. 2005;25(6):339–356. [ Google Scholar ]
- Edmondson R. A social interpretation of personal wisdom. In: Ferrari M, Weststrate NM, editors. The scientific study of personal wisdom. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 191–209. [ Google Scholar ]
- Freudenthaler HH, Neubauer AC. Emotional intelligence: The convergent and discriminant validities of intra-and interpersonal emotional abilities. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39:569–579. [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück J, Baltes PB. Using the concept of wisdom to enhance the expression of wisdom knowledge: Not the philosopher’s dream but differential effects of developmental preparedness. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21:679–690. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.679. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück J, Bluck S. Laypeople’s conceptions of wisdom and its development: Cognitive and integrative views. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2011;66:321–324. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr011. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück J, Bluck S. The MORE life experience model: A theory of the development of personal wisdom. In: Ferrari M, Weststrate NM, editors. The scientific study of personal wisdom: From contemplative traditions to neuroscience. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer; 2013. pp. 75–97. [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück J, Bluck S, Baron J, McAdams DP. The wisdom of experience: Autobiographical narratives across adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29:197–208. [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück J, König S, Naschenweng K, Redzanowski U, Dorner L, Strasser I, Wiedermann W. How to measure wisdom: Content, reliability, and validity of five measures. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00405. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Glück, J. 2018. Measuring wisdom: existing approaches, continuing challenges, and new developments. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences (advance access). [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ]
- Glück, J. in press. The development of wisdom during adulthood. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Glück (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Wisdom . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 3–24. [ Google Scholar ]
- Grossmann I. Wisdom in context. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2017;12(2):233–257. doi: 10.1177/1745691616672066. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Igarashi, H., M.R. Levenson, and C.M. Aldwin. 2018. The development of wisdom: A social ecological approach. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences (advance access). [ DOI ] [ PubMed ]
- Jeste DV, Ardelt M, Blazer D, Kraemer HC, Vaillant G, Meeks TW. Expert consensus on characteristics of wisdom: A Delphi method study. The Gerontologist. 2010;50:668–680. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq022. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Joseph S, Linley PA. Positive adjustment to threatening events: An organismic valuing theory of growth through adversity. Review of General Psychology. 2005;9:262–280. [ Google Scholar ]
- Jung, C.G. 1971. Mensch und Seele. Aus dem Gesamtwerk 1905-1961. In Mensch und Seele. Aus dem Gesamtwerk 1905-1961 , Hrsg. J. Jacobi, and C.G. Jung. Olten: Walter-Verlag.
- King LA, Scollon CK, Ramsey C, Williams T. Stories of life transition: Subjective well-being and ego development in parents of children with Down Syndrome. Journal of Research in Personality. 2000;34:509–536. [ Google Scholar ]
- Kinnier RT, Tribbensee NE, Rose CA, Vaughan SM. In the final analysis: More wisdom from people who have faced death. Journal of Counseling & Development. 2001;79:171–177. [ Google Scholar ]
- Koller I, Levenson MR, Glück J. What do you think you are measuring? A mixed-methods procedure for assessing the content validity of test items and theory-based scaling. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00126. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- König S, Glück J. “Gratitude is with me all the time:” How gratitude relates to wisdom. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences. 2014;69:655–666. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt123. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Kozulin A. Dynamic assessment in search of its identity. In: Yasnitsky A, van der Veer R, Ferrari M, editors. The Cambridge handbook of cultural-historical psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2014. pp. 126–147. [ Google Scholar ]
- Kramer DA. Conceptualizing wisdom: The primacy of affect-cognition relations. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (S. 279-131) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. [ Google Scholar ]
- Kramer DA. The ontogeny of wisdom in its variations. In: Demick J, Andreotti C, editors. Handbook of adult development. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2003. pp. 131–151. [ Google Scholar ]
- Kunzmann U. Approaches to a good life: The emotional-motivational side to wisdom. In: Linley PA, Joseph S, editors. Positive psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons; 2004. pp. 504–517. [ Google Scholar ]
- Labouvie-Vief, G. 1990. Wisdom as integrated thought: Historical and developmental perspectives. In Wisdom. Its nature, origins, and development (S. 52-83), ed. R. J. Sternberg. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Levenson MR. Gender and wisdom: The roles of compassion and moral development. Research in Human Development. 2009;6:45–59. [ Google Scholar ]
- Levenson MR, Jennings PA, Aldwin CM, Shiraishi RW. Self-transcendence: Conceptualization and measurement. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2005;60:127–143. doi: 10.2190/XRXM-FYRA-7U0X-GRC0. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Lilgendahl J, McAdams DP. Constructing stories of self-growth: How individual differences in patterns of autobiographical reasoning relate to well-being in midlife. Journal of Personality. 2011;79:391–428. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00688.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Linley PA. Positive adaptation to trauma: Wisdom as both process and outcome. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2003;16:601–610. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000004086.64509.09. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Linley PA, Joseph S. Positive change following trauma and adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2004;17:11–21. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- McKee P, Barber C. On defining wisdom. International Journal of Human Development. 1999;49:149–164. doi: 10.2190/8G32-BNV0-NVP9-7V6G. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Mickler C, Staudinger UM. Personal wisdom: Validation and age-related differences of a performance measure. Psychology and Aging. 2008;23:787–799. doi: 10.1037/a0013928. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Pals JL. Narrative identity processing of difficult life experiences: Pathways of personality development and positive self-transformation in adulthood. Journal of Personality. 2006;74:1080–1109. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00403.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Pascual-Leone J. An essay on wisdom: Toward organismic processes that make it possible. In: Sternberg RJ, editor. Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 13–24. [ Google Scholar ]
- Peterson C, Bossio LM. Optimism and physical well-being. In: Chang EC, editor. Optimism & pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2001. pp. 127–145. [ Google Scholar ]
- Rathunde K. Experiential wisdom and optimal experience: Interviews with three distinguished lifelong learners. Journal of Adult Development. 2010;17:81–93. [ Google Scholar ]
- Rogers CR. Toward a modern approach to values: The valuing process in the mature person. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1964;68:160–167. doi: 10.1037/h0046419. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Seligman MEP. Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman; 1975. [ Google Scholar ]
- Staudinger UM. Life reflection: A social-cognitive analysis of life review. Review of General Psychology. 2001;5:148–160. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.148. [ DOI ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Staudinger UM, Lopez DF, Baltes PB. The psychometric location of wisdom-related performance: Intelligence, personality, and more? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1997;23:1200–1214. [ Google Scholar ]
- Staudinger UM, Smith J, Baltes PB. Manual for the assessment of wisdom-related knowledge. Berlin, Germany: Max Planck Institute for Human Development; 1994. [ Google Scholar ]
- Sternberg RJ. A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General Psychology. 1998;2:347–365. [ Google Scholar ]
- Taylor SE, Brown JD. Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin. 1988;103:193–210. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry. 2004;15:1–18. [ Google Scholar ]
- Troy AS, Wilhelm FH, Shallcross AJ, Mauss IB. Seeing the silver lining: Cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion. 2010;10:783–795. doi: 10.1037/a0020262. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Wagnild GM, Young HM. Development and psychometric evaluation of the Resilience Scale. Journal of Nursing Measurement. 1993;1:165–178. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Webster JD. An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale. Journal of Adult Development. 2003;10:13–22. [ Google Scholar ]
- Webster JD. Measuring the character strength of wisdom. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 2007;65:163–183. doi: 10.2190/AG.65.2.d. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Weststrate NM, Ferrari M, Ardelt M. The many faces of wisdom: An investigation of cultural-historical wisdom exemplars reveals practical, philosophical, and benevolent prototypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2016;42(5):662–676. doi: 10.1177/0146167216638075. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Weststrate NM, Ferrari M, Fournier MA, McLean KC. “It was the best worst day of my life”: narrative content, structure, and process in wisdom-fostering life event memories. Psychological Sciences (advance access): The Journals of Gerontology Series B; 2018. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- Weststrate NM, Glück J. Hard-earned wisdom: Exploratory processing of difficult life experience is positively associated with wisdom. Developmental Psychology. 2017;53:800–814. doi: 10.1037/dev0000286. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
- View on publisher site
- PDF (979.9 KB)
- Collections
Similar articles
Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.
- Download .nbib .nbib
- Format: AMA APA MLA NLM
Add to Collections
Home — Essay Samples — Life — Life Experiences — Personal Experience
Personal Experience Essays
Personal experiences are the threads that weave the fabric of our lives. Writing a personal experience essay isn't just about storytelling; it's about finding meaning, connecting with others, and leaving your mark on the world. So, why should you write an essay about your personal experiences? Let's explore the importance together! 🌟
Personal Experience Essay Topics 📝
Selecting the right essay topic is key to crafting a compelling narrative. Here's how to pick one:
Personal Experience Argumentative Essay 🤨
Argumentative essays based on personal experiences require you to defend a viewpoint or argument. Here are ten intriguing topics:
- 1. Argue for or against the idea that personal experiences are the most influential factors shaping an individual's personality.
- 2. Defend your perspective on whether overcoming adversity through personal experiences builds stronger character.
- 3. Debate the impact of personal experiences on shaping one's political beliefs and values.
- 4. Argue for the significance of sharing personal experiences in order to promote empathy and understanding among diverse communities.
- 5. Defend the idea that personal experiences play a crucial role in career development and decision-making.
- 6. Debate the ethical implications of sharing deeply personal experiences in the era of social media and oversharing.
- 7. Argue for the therapeutic benefits of writing about and reflecting on personal experiences.
- 8. Defend your perspective on whether personal experiences should be a central part of school curricula.
- 9. Debate the influence of personal experiences on an individual's approach to health and wellness.
- 10. Argue for or against the notion that personal experiences can serve as catalysts for social change and activism.
Personal Experience Cause and Effect Essay 🤯
Cause and effect essays based on personal experiences explore the reasons behind events and their consequences. Here are ten topics to consider:
- 1. Analyze the causes and effects of a life-changing personal experience on your academic or career choices.
- 2. Examine how personal experiences can lead to personal growth, increased self-awareness, and improved well-being.
- 3. Investigate the effects of travel experiences on personal perspectives and cultural understanding.
- 4. Analyze the causes and consequences of sharing personal experiences with others, including its impact on relationships.
- 5. Examine how personal experiences can influence one's hobbies, interests, and leisure activities.
- 6. Investigate the impact of a significant personal experience on your family dynamics and relationships.
- 7. Analyze the causes of personal transformation through exposure to diverse cultures and environments.
- 8. Examine how personal experiences can shape one's attitude toward risk-taking and adventure.
- 9. Investigate the effects of sharing personal experiences through writing, art, or storytelling on your personal well-being.
- 10. Analyze the causes and consequences of personal experiences that challenge societal norms and expectations.
Personal Experience Opinion Essay 😌
Opinion essays based on personal experiences allow you to express your subjective viewpoints. Here are ten topics to consider:
- 1. Share your opinion on the importance of documenting personal experiences for future generations.
- 2. Discuss your perspective on whether personal experiences should be kept private or shared openly.
- 3. Express your thoughts on how personal experiences have shaped your sense of identity and self-worth.
- 4. Debate the significance of personal experiences in fostering empathy and compassion among individuals and communities.
- 5. Share your views on the role of personal experiences in building resilience and coping with life's challenges.
- 6. Discuss the impact of personal experiences on your approach to decision-making and problem-solving.
- 7. Express your opinion on the therapeutic benefits of writing or talking about personal experiences.
- 8. Debate the influence of personal experiences on your sense of purpose and life goals.
- 9. Share your perspective on how personal experiences can inspire creativity and artistic expression.
- 10. Discuss your favorite personal experience and the lessons or insights it has provided.
Personal Experience Informative Essay 🧐
Informative essays based on personal experiences aim to educate readers. Here are ten informative topics to explore:
- 1. Provide an in-depth analysis of the impact of a specific personal experience on your career choices and aspirations.
- 2. Explore the therapeutic benefits of journaling and writing about personal experiences for mental health and well-being.
- 3. Investigate the history and significance of storytelling as a means of preserving personal experiences and cultural heritage.
- 4. Analyze the connection between personal experiences and the development of emotional intelligence.
- 5. Examine the influence of personal experiences on decision-making processes and risk assessment.
- 6. Investigate the role of personal experiences in shaping cultural perceptions and worldviews.
- 7. Provide insights into the art of crafting compelling narratives based on personal experiences.
- 8. Analyze the impact of personal experiences on an individual's resilience and ability to adapt to change.
- 9. Examine how personal experiences can serve as valuable life lessons and sources of wisdom.
- 10. Investigate the therapeutic benefits of group discussions and support networks for individuals sharing similar personal experiences.
Personal Experience Essay Example 📄
Personal experience thesis statement examples 📜.
Here are five examples of strong thesis statements for your personal experience essay:
- 1. "Through the lens of personal experiences, we uncover the profound impact that seemingly ordinary moments can have on our lives, reshaping our perspectives and guiding our journeys."
- 2. "Personal experiences serve as powerful mirrors reflecting our growth, resilience, and capacity to navigate life's challenges, ultimately shaping the narratives of our existence."
- 3. "The sharing of personal experiences is an act of vulnerability and courage, fostering connections, empathy, and a deeper understanding of the human condition."
- 4. "Our personal experiences are the brushstrokes on the canvas of our identity, influencing our choices, values, and the stories we tell ourselves and others."
- 5. "In exploring personal experiences, we embark on a journey of self-discovery, unlocking the untold stories that shape our uniqueness and enrich our shared human tapestry."
Personal Experience Essay Introduction Examples 🚀
Here are three captivating introduction paragraphs to kickstart your essay:
- 1. "Amid the chaos of everyday life, our personal experiences are the constellations that guide us, the moments that define us. As we embark on this essay journey into the depths of our own stories, we unravel the threads of our existence, each tale a testament to the power of the personal."
- 2. "Picture a canvas where the brushstrokes are the chapters of your life—a canvas waiting for you to paint your experiences, thoughts, and emotions. The personal experience essay is your opportunity to create a masterpiece that reflects the colors of your journey."
- 3. "In a world of noise and distractions, our personal experiences are the melodies that resonate within us. As we venture into the heart of this essay, we uncover the symphony of our lives—a composition of highs, lows, and the beauty in between."
Personal Experience Conclusion Examples 🌟
Conclude your essay with impact using these examples:
- 1. "As we close the chapter on this exploration of personal experiences, we are reminded that our stories are the threads that connect us all. The journey continues, and each experience, no matter how small, contributes to the tapestry of our shared humanity."
- 2. "In the final brushstroke of our personal experience essay, we recognize that our stories are not finite; they are ever-evolving, ever-inspiring. The canvas of life awaits, ready for us to create new narratives and continue shaping our destinies."
- 3. "As the echoes of our personal experiences linger, we stand at the intersection of past, present, and future. The essay's conclusion is but a pause in the symphony of our lives, with countless more notes to be played and stories to be written."
Identity: a Student's Journey of Self-discovery
A scholar's journey: lessons, growth, and aspirations, made-to-order essay as fast as you need it.
Each essay is customized to cater to your unique preferences
+ experts online
An Unforgettable Experience in My Life
Personal background: my life story as a definition of me, personal experience that made me better than before, motivation through failure: my life experience, let us write you an essay from scratch.
- 450+ experts on 30 subjects ready to help
- Custom essay delivered in as few as 3 hours
The Role of Memorable Memories in Our Lives
The time my uncle died, the importance and role of hard work and efforts in your success, my most embarrassing moment, get a personalized essay in under 3 hours.
Expert-written essays crafted with your exact needs in mind
Memories of Happiness and Accomplishments in My Life
Life experience that made me who i am, how i have learnt to appreciate people, the importance of positive people around you, a narrative about regrets in life, the influence of grandmother on my life, mistakes made and lessons learned, my experience in learning to read and write, personal writing: my childhood story, personal writing: experience at the mall, basketball court – my second home, the lessons i have learnt from my life experience, the best travel experience of my life - burma, changing my view of life, the beginning of my nightmare: first day of high school, a personal experience of a course of multicultural education, the lessons i’ve learned in middle school, shyness is not always a curse, the benefits of community service experience for me, relevant topics.
- Law of Life
- Life Changing Experience
- Professionalism
- Overcoming Obstacles
- Why I Want To Be A Nurse
- Personal Growth and Development
- Overcoming Challenges
- Volunteering
- Childhood Memories
By clicking “Check Writers’ Offers”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy . We’ll occasionally send you promo and account related email
No need to pay just yet!
Bibliography
We use cookies to personalyze your web-site experience. By continuing we’ll assume you board with our cookie policy .
- Instructions Followed To The Letter
- Deadlines Met At Every Stage
- Unique And Plagiarism Free
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
The MORE Life Experience Model argues that people higher in a sense of mastery, openness, reflectivity, emotion regulation, and empathy are more likely than others both to display wisdom in dealing with life challenges and to continue growing towards greater wisdom across a lifetime.
In this study, we focused on the 10 major life events that have been studied most often: Entering a new relationship, marriage, birth of a child, separation, divorce, widowhood, graduation, entering the first job, unemployment, and retirement.
Phenomenology is all about understanding the lived experience of others. It is at once a philosophy, a perspective, and an approach to research.
Having lived experience identified roles in research, such as co-investigators or advisers, allowed people to give voice to their own and others’ experiences, to shape research relevant to people’s lives, and not feel misrepresented in research outputs.
Through a cross-sectional retrospective design, 394 adults from the community answered the Lifetime Experiences Scale, which covers 75 life experiences organized in eight domains (i.e., school, job, health, leisure, living conditions, adverse experiences, achievements, and people and relationships).
Research Articles. Life experience pathways to college student emotional and mental health: A structural equation model. Carl L. Hanson. , PhD, MCHES, Brianna M. Magnusson. , PhD, Alice Ann Crandall. , PhD, Michael D. Barnes. , PhD, MCHES, Emily McFarland. , BS & McKaylee Smith. , BS.
Life experiences have been a topic of interest for researchers and clinicians for decades. Current knowledge is rooted in two distinct approaches, i.e., personality psychology and psychosomatics.
In this study, we interviewed 150 people in 22 countries on six continents, asking them, ‘What do you love?’, ‘What is a great passion in your life?’, and ‘What has been a peak experience in...
In particular, lived experience designs seek to answer how, what, and why questions to develop meaning or theory from the context and the people who lived the phenomenon. We review four lived experience traditions: phenomenology, consensual qualitative research, narrative inquiry, and case study.
Personal experiences are the threads that weave the fabric of our lives. Writing a personal experience essay isn't just about storytelling; it's about finding meaning, connecting with others, and leaving your mark on the world.