• Open access
  • Published: 17 June 2021

Relationship between sleep habits and academic performance in university Nursing students

  • Juana Inés Gallego-Gómez 1 ,
  • María Teresa Rodríguez González-Moro 1 ,
  • José Miguel Rodríguez González-Moro 2 ,
  • Tomás Vera-Catalán 1 ,
  • Serafín Balanza 1 ,
  • Agustín Javier Simonelli-Muñoz 3 &
  • José Miguel Rivera-Caravaca 4  

BMC Nursing volume  20 , Article number:  100 ( 2021 ) Cite this article

122k Accesses

22 Citations

3 Altmetric

Metrics details

Sleep disorders are composed of a group of diseases of increasing prevalence and with social-health implications to be considered a public health problem. Sleep habits and specific sleep behaviors have an influence on the academic success of students. However, the characteristics of sleep and sleep habits of university students as predictors of poor academic performance have been scarcely analyzed. In the present study, we aimed to investigate sleep habits and their influence on academic performance in a cohort of Nursing Degree students.

This was a cross-sectional and observational study. An anonymous and self-administered questionnaire was used, including different scales such as the ‘Morningness and Eveningness scale’, an author-generated sleep habit questionnaire, and certain variables aimed at studying the socio-familial and academic aspects of the Nursing students. The association of sleep habits and other variables with poor academic performance was investigated by logistic regression. The internal consistency and homogeneity of the ‘sleep habits questionnaire’ was assessed with the Cronbach’s alpha test.

Overall, 401 students (mean age of 22.1 ± 4.9 years, 74.8 % females) from the Nursing Degree were included. The homogeneity of the ‘sleep habits questionnaire’ was appropriate (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.710). Nursing students were characterized by an evening chronotype (20.2 %) and a short sleep pattern. 30.4 % of the Nursing students had bad sleep habits. Regarding the academic performance, 47.9 % of the students showed a poor one. On multivariate logistic regression analysis, a short sleep pattern (adjusted OR = 1.53, 95 % CI 1.01–2.34), bad sleep habits (aOR = 1.76, 95 % CI 1.11–2.79), and age < 25 years (aOR = 2.27, 95 % CI 1.30–3.98) were independently associated with a higher probability of poor academic performance.

Conclusions

Almost 1/3 of the Nursing students were identified as having bad sleep habits, and these students were characterized by an evening chronotype and a short sleep pattern. A short sleep pattern, bad sleep habits, and age < 25 years, were independently associated with a higher risk of poor academic performance. This requires multifactorial approaches and the involvement of all the associated actors: teachers, academic institutions, health institutions, and the people in charge in university residences, among others.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Sleep is a complex phenomenon resulting from the interaction between the neuroendocrine system, biological clock and biochemical processes, with environmental, social and cultural aspects that are very relevant in the life stages of adolescence and youth [ 1 ]. Indeed, the chronic lack of sleep is a recent worry among adolescents and young university students and it is associated with worse health and clinical outcomes [ 2 , 3 ].

Among biological factors determining sleep, there are “chronotypes” and sleep patterns. The first term refers to the personal preferences of scheduling the sleep-wake cycle, emphasizing three basic chronotypes: morning (early-risers), and evening (night-owls) and those who are intermediate, defined as those who do not have clear preferences towards any of the extreme schedules for the fulfilling of their activities [ 4 ]. The sleep pattern refers to the personal schedule of bedtime and wake-up time. In this sense, a circadian rhythm is a natural, internal process, driven by a circadian clock that repeats roughly every 24 h and regulates the sleep-wake cycle [ 5 ].

On the other hand, the sleep habits are in the intersection between biological and cultural values. Endogenous, exogenous or environmental factors are included here, as well as those activities that are developed by the population to induce or maintain sleep, with its study and care becoming a challenge for Nursing [ 6 ]. Currently, spontaneous abusive behaviors regarding sleep habits are becoming frequent, leading to a state of chronic sleep deprivation, which translates to fatigue and somnolence during the day [ 7 ]. Hence, there is a high prevalence of sleep disorders in university students, especially those that affect the wake-sleep rhythm [ 2 ]. For this reason,the interest in establishing relationships between sleep and cognitive processes such as memory, learning ability and motivation, has gained attention during the last years. However, studies that relate sleep with academic problems are scarce, despite previous authors have shown that the reduction of sleep time in teenagers and university students was associated with poor academic performance, accidents and obesity [ 8 , 9 ]. Since good-quality sleep does not only imply sleeping well at night but also an adequate level of attention during the day for performing different tasks, appropriate sleep has an influence in efficient learning processes in university students [ 10 , 11 , 12 ].

Although some scientific evidence has shown a relationship between sleep and low academic performance [ 13 , 14 ], so far, there are no questionnaires to specifically evaluate sleep habits in Nursing students. Considering that this population has special characteristics, they are mostly young, combine hospital training at the same time they attend classes at the university, they present lifestyles that can negatively influence the academic performance. To study the sleep habits using a specific tool, in addition to analyze the sleep pattern and chronotype, could help to identify students with inappropriate sleep habits for developing interventions to modify these habits. This might have a positive impact on their academic performance and avoid potentially serious negative consequences for their physical and mental health. In the present research, we aimed (a) to design a ‘sleep habits questionnaire’, (b) to analyze the sleep habits, sleep pattern and chronotype, and (c) to investigate sleep habits and their influence on academic performance, in a cohort of Nursing Degree students.

Design and study population

This was an observational, prospective and cross-sectional study involving Nursing students, all of them distributed among the 4 years of the Nursing Degree. There were no inclusion criteria, i.e. all Nursing students were suitable for the study, unless those who did not attend class on the day of data collection, or those who did not wish to participate (from 420 students, 19 refused to participate in the study). The study was fully carried out during the first semester of the 2019–2020 academic year.

Study Variables

Circadian rhythm: the reduced “horne & östberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire”.

Preferences of schedule for the sleep-wake cycle and its influence on academic performance were assessed using the reduced version of the Horne & Östberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (rMEQ) proposed by Adan & Almirall [ 15 ], translated to Spanish, that is composed of 5 items. The score determines the following five types of schedule: clearly morning type (22–25 points), moderately morning type (18–21 points), no preference (12–17 points), moderately evening type (8–11 points), and clearly evening type (4–7 points). The internal consistency of the circadian rhythm scale assessed using the rMEQ by Adan & Almirall is good, as the scores from all the items are correlated among themselves [ 15 , 16 ].

Sleep habits questionnaire

For the initial design of the sleep habits questionnaire, a panel of 10 voluntary experts was included. This panel was composed of 5 registered nurses and 5 physicians, with a minimum of 5 years of experience in sleep. All of them were interviewed and informed individually about the study. Items composing of the questionnaire were obtained according to the scientific literature and the main factors influencing sleep habits as the discretion of the expert panel [ 14 , 17 , 18 ]. Eleven questions were finally included in a self-reported questionnaire, each ranging from 1 to 4 (never (1), sometimes (2), usually (3), always (4)) ( Supplementary file ). Sleep habits, including sleep routines, study schedule preference, and napping were also evaluated. The overall score of the questionnaire ranges from 11 to 44 points, with the highest scores indicating the worst sleep habits. As there is no specific cut-off point for this questionnaire, students over the fourth quartile (4Q, i.e. ≥25 points) were categorized as having inappropriate habits. Therefore, these Nursing students were included in the “bad sleeping habits” group.

  • Academic performance

The academic performance was measured by the ratio “failed exams/performed exams” and checked in the student’s academic records. A good academic performance was considered if the final grade of every exam completed during the Nursing Degree was ≥ 5 (in a 0–10 range, where an exam is considered passed if the score is ≥ 5).

Other variables

Other variables such as gender, age and hours of sleep (sleep pattern), were analyzed. To describe the sleep pattern of the Nursing students, we used the classification described by Miró et al. (2002) [ 19 ]. This classification was composed of three categories as a function of the hours slept, so that we found subjects that had a short sleep pattern (< 6 h per day), subjects with a long sleep pattern (≥ 9 h per day), and subjects with an intermediate sleep pattern (6–9 h per day).

Ethical considerations

The study protocol was approved by an accredited Ethics Committee (Reference: CE-6191) and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. All students were informed and gave consent to participation in the study. The anonymity and confidentiality were guaranteed.

Statistical analysis

The sample size was calculated by a non-probabilistic sampling technique using Ene 2.0 (GlaxoSmithKline) with a precision ± 5 % and α error = 0.05. This calculation was based on the estimation that the prevalence of bad sleep habits in Nursing students of our university was 30.4 %, which resulted in a minimum sample of 229 subjects.

Categorical variables were expressed as frequencies and percentages. Continuous variables were presented as mean ± standard deviation (SD) or median and interquartile range (IQR), as appropriate.

The Pearson Chi-squared test was used to compare proportions whereas comparison of continuous variables was performed using the Student t test. Correlations between different scales were performed using the Pearson’s correlation test.

In order to investigate if sleep habits and other variables were independently associated with poor academic performance, a logistic regression model (with odds ratios [OR] and two-sided 95 % confidence intervals [CI]) was performed. To measure the internal consistency and homogeneity of the sleep habits questionnaire, the Cronbach’s alpha test was performed.

A p -value < 0.05 was accepted as statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v. 21.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

We included 401 Nursing students (100 students from 1st year, 105 from 2nd year, 101 from 3rd year, and 95 from 4th year) in the study. The students were characterized for being predominantly females (300, 74.8 %), with a mean age of 22.1 ± 4.9 years, and the majority of them (88.5 %) were singles.

Sleep habits of the Nursing students were examined using our previously designed (as described in the Methods section) self-reported ‘sleep habits questionnaire’. The homogeneity of the questionnaire was appropriate, with a Cronbach’s alpha value of 0.710. The mean score in the questionnaire was 22.3 ± 3.9, and 30.4 % of the Nursing students had bad sleep habits (i.e. score > 4Q), which were characterized by a clear preference of studying at night, easily lose a night of sleep for work-related or academic tasks that imply staying up late, and showing difficulties in maintaining sleep routines.

Table  1 shows the summarized results for each question of the sleep habits questionnaire.

The Nursing students in our sample were characterized by an evening chronotype (20.2 %, 81) and a short sleep pattern (i.e. <6 h of sleep daily), with 51.1 % (205) of the students sleeping less than 6 h/day, 42.1 % (169) sleeping 6–9 h/day, and 6.7 % (27) sleeping more than 9 h/day. The mean duration of sleep found in the Nursing students was 6.52 ± 1.4 h.

Of note, most of the Nursing students that had an evening chronotype were < 25 years old (22.2 %, p  = 0.011). In addition, age showed a positive association with circadian rhythm and as age increased, the students tended to have a predominantly morning chronotype ( R  = 0.223, p  < 0.001). Nursing students < 25 years of age had also worse sleep habits according to the sleep habits questionnaire than those ≥ 25 years (22.61 ± 3.79 vs. 21.19 ± 4.37, p  = 0.005). A negative correlation was found between the overall sleep habits questionnaire score and age as a continuous variable ( R = -0.105, p  = 0.03).

In addition, 29.5 % of patients that had bad sleep habits ( p  = 0.001), and 23.9 % that had poor academic performance ( p  = 0.020), had also an evening chronotype (Table  2 ). A significant negative correlation was found between the sleep pattern and sleep habits ( R = -0.293, p  < 0.001), and between circadian rhythm and sleep habits, hence Nursing students with good sleep habits have predominantly a morning circadian rhythm ( R = -0.201, p  < 0.001).

Regarding the academic performance, 93 % (373) of the Nursing students attended all the exams planned, and 47.9 % (192) of the students showed poor academic performance. When we investigated specifically if the sleep habits, as assessed by the ‘sleep habits questionnaire’, influenced the academic performance, we found that 32 % (140) of the Nursing students that had bad sleep habits obtained poor academic results ( p  < 0.001). Those that had the worst academic results were the ones that did not have a regular hour for waking up and going to sleep (2.66 ± 1.03, p  = 0.031), presented difficulties to maintain the sleep during the night (1.73 ± 0.77, p  = 0.003), and preferred to study for an exam at night (1.33 ± 0.48, p  = 0.030), as well as going to bed late to obtain better results (1.46 ± 0.51, p  = 0.041). Also, those students with poorer academic results where those listening to music before going to bed (1.84 ± 1.10, p  = 0.007), and going out at night even if they had to get-up early the next day (1.58 ± 0.72, p  = 0.012). Overall, those Nursing students whose work or academic activities entailed going to bed late to attain their objectives, had the lowest academic performance (2.25 ± 1.01, p  = 0.001). Lastly, we can confirm that the Nursing students that had better academic performance were the ones who had the best sleep habits. Indeed, the overall ‘sleep habits questionnaire’ score was significantly lower compared to those Nursing students who had poor academic performance (21.91 ± 3.90 vs. 24.18 ± 3.55, p  < 0.001) (Table  3 ).

Finally, the profile of Nursing students with more failed courses was characterized by an evening circadian rhythm ( R = -0.134, p  = 0.007), bad sleep habits ( R  = 0.216, p  < 0.001), and less hours of sleep daily ( R = -0.211, p  < 0.001).

To confirm these observations, a multivariate logistic regression analysis was performed. Therefore, a short sleep pattern (adjusted OR = 1.53, 95 % CI 1.01–2.34), bad sleep habits (adjusted OR = 1.76, 95 % CI 1.11–2.79), and age < 25 years (adjusted OR = 2.27, 95 % CI 1.30–3.98) were independently associated with a higher probability of poor academic performance (Table  4 ).

Sleep is an excellent indicator of the health status and an element that favors good quality of life [ 20 ], but entering university is a change that highly impacts the student in every dimension, including sleep habits [ 21 , 22 ]. A potential barrier for maximizing performance during the university stage is the irregular sleep schedule, which lead to sleep deficit and high prevalence of somnolence during the day [ 23 ]. A review by Shochat et al. (2014) [ 24 ] examined the consequences of lack of sleep among Nursing students, and confirmed the relationship between sleep disorders and changes in sleep patterns with a reduced academic performance. Other studies have established that sleep has an integral role in learning and memory consolidation [ 25 , 26 ]. Therefore, despite some scientific evidence has shown a relationship between sleep and low academic performance [ 13 , 14 ], the originality of our study was to examine the influence that sleep characteristics exert (chronotypes and sleep patterns), as well as sleep habits of the university population on academic performance.

Overall, the academic performance of our Nursing students was suboptimal. When analyzing how sleep pattern, sleep habits, and circadian rhythms influenced this academic performance, we observed that all of them may be determine factors for learning, as other studies have done [ 27 ].

Concerning the sleep pattern, it should be noted that most of the students enrolled in the Nursing Degree slept less than 6 h per day. Of note, our results seem to establish a relationship between the hours slept and the academic performance during the first semester, as gathered from the academic records. This finding is in accordance to observations by other authors in university students from Medicine [ 9 ], Pharmacy [ 2 ] or Nursing [ 28 ], which also showed evidence between the hours slept and the academic achievement. In a previous study, we already observed that university students from the Faculty of Nursing attributed the hours slept with academic performance [ 29 ]. Indeed, it should be highlighted that chronic lack of sleep is not only associated with alterations of attention and academic performance, but also to a series of adverse consequences for health such as risky behaviors, depression, anxiety, alterations in social relations, and obesity, among others [ 30 ].

In addition, our study has evidenced how the sleep habits directly influenced the academic performance of these Nursing students, and approximately 1/3 of the students with bad sleep habits obtained poor academic results. Certainly, the sleep pattern and inadequate sleep habits could be related. Good sleep hygiene includes aspects such as a regular sleep-wake schedule, adequate environment, avoiding stimulating activities before going to bed, and limiting the use of technology in bed or immediately before going to bed. In the present study, 30.4 % of the students had bad sleep habits, characterized by having a clear preference for studying at night, often losing a night of sleep for work or academic activities that imply go to bed late, and show difficulties in maintaining sleep routines. An important proportion of our Nursing degree students declared that they watched television, listened to music, worked or read academic documents during the last hour before going to bed. In this sense, LeBourgeois et al. (2017) [ 31 ] have described the university population as great consumers of technology, and have associated the frequent use of technology before going to bed with problems to sleep and daytime somnolence.

Finally, age was another factor that should be considered in the analysis of sleep habits. According to our results, the Nursing students that were < 25 years of age had the worst sleep habits and used to have more difficulties in maintaining sleep routines, modifying them on the weekends and holidays, preferring to stay up late to obtain better study results, and going out at night without considering that they had to get up early. As other studies [ 21 ], we observed that social activities were a priority in the life of the university adolescents and the substituting of hours of sleep for enjoying and sharing activities with friends and classmates did not constitute a problem for them. These behaviors were added to the physiological delay of the start of sleep that is typical in this stage of life and might unleash deprivation or a chronic deficit of sleep, maintained throughout the entire week. The students then tried to compensate for this lack of sleep by increasing their hours of sleep during the weekend. We agree with previous studies that this circumstance, far from minimizing or compensating the effects of sleep deprivation, aggravates them, worsening the pattern and the quality of sleep of the students [ 22 ].

Further, we found an association between age and circadian type. We observed that most of the university students with evening chronotypes were aged < 25, had bad sleep habits, and a poor academic performance. Physiologically, adolescents and adults tend to have delayed circadian preferences and are “lovers of the night” [ 23 ]. In our study, 20.2 % of students had an evening chronotype, which is lower than that reported in other studies, where 59 % of the students between 18 and 29 years of age described themselves as night owls [ 32 ]. Our results also showed a clear normalization of the evening behaviors of the students. These data are in agreement with other authors who highlighted the influence exerted by the aforementioned normalization of evening habits among the youth on the quality of sleep, leading to a medium to long-term sleep deficit [ 20 ]. As Crowley et al. (2018) [ 33 ], we think that evening behavior leads to asynchrony between the biological rhythm and the social life of the student, having negative consequences on the academic performance. However, how this really affects academic results requires extending researches, since the circadian rhythm was not significantly associated with academic performance.

The results of this study evidence the need to seriously take into consideration the sleep deficits that are associated with inadequate sleep habits, with the aim of developing preventative and educational initiatives to improve the sleep habits of the university population. The challenge ahead starts with the social awareness of the importance of having good-quality sleep since many times, adequate knowledge about sleep does not translate into a change of sleep habits [ 23 ].

Limitations

Some limitations should be noted. Due to the cross-sectional design of the study, we could not establish an exact causal relationship between sleep pattern and academic performance. In addition, it should be note that the ‘sleep habits questionnaire’ is a subjective questionnaire, and therefore the result could be biased if the student did not answer honestly. Another limitation is the difficulty in conceptualizing academic performance, due to its complex and multi-causal character, where many factors intervene. The factors include attitudes, habits, the character of the staff, methodologies, family environment, organization of the educational system, socio-economic condition, as well as other social, economic, and psychological aspects [ 34 ]. Finally, the study was conducted only in Nursing students, so our results must be prospectively validated in University students from a larger variety of academic sectors. Similarly, this study was conducted in a single University, so more studies involving other Universities are also necessary. Despite these circumstances, we believe that our hypothesis that the duration of sleep could lead to better academic performance is based on current scientific data.

Using the 11-item ‘sleep habits questionnaire’, 30.4 % of the Nursing students were identified as having bad sleep habits. In addition, Nursing students included in this research were characterized by an evening chronotype and a short sleep pattern. Regarding academic performance, half of the Nursing students showed a poor one. A short sleep pattern, bad sleep habits, and younger age, were independently associated with a higher risk of poor academic performance. This requires multifactorial approaches and the involvement of all the associated actors: teachers, academic institutions, health institutions, and the people in charge in university residences, among others.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Matricciani L, Bin YS, Lallukka T, Kronholm E, Wake M, Paquet C, Dumuid D, Olds T. Rethinking the sleep-health link. Sleep Health. 2018;4(4):339–348. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.05.004 .

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Zeek ML, Savoie MJ, Song M, Kennemur LM, Qian J, Jungnickel PW, Westrick SC. Sleep Duration and Academic Performance Among Student Pharmacists. Am J Pharm Educ. 2015;79(5):63. doi: https://doi.org/10.5688/ajpe79563 .

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Dijk DJ, Landolt HP. Sleep Physiology, Circadian Rhythms, Waking Performance and the Development of Sleep-Wake Therapeutics. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2019;253:441–481. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/164_2019_243 .

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Zerbini G, Merrow M. Time to learn: How chronotype impacts education. Psych J. 2017;6(4):263–276. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.178 .

Huang W, Ramsey KM, Marcheva B, Bass J. Circadian rhythms, sleep, and metabolism. J Clin Invest. 2011;121(6):2133–41. doi: https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI46043 .

Owens H, Christian B, Polivka B. Sleep behaviors in traditional-age college students: A state of the science review with implications for practice. J Am Assoc Nurse Pract. 2017; 29(11):695–703. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/2327-6924.12520 .

Becerra MB, Bol BS, Granados R, Hassija C. Sleepless in school: The role of social determinants of sleep health among college students. J Am Coll Health. 2020; 68(2):185–191. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2018.1538148 .

Kozak AT, Pickett SM, Jarrett NL, Markarian SA, Lahar KI, Goldstick JE. Project STARLIT: protocol of a longitudinal study of habitual sleep trajectories, weight gain, and obesity risk behaviors in college students. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):1720. doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-019-7697-x .

El Hangouche AJ, Jniene A, Aboudrar S, Errguig L, Rkain H, Cherti M, Dakka T. Relationship between poor sleep quality, excessive daytime sleepiness and poor academic performance in medical students. Adv Med Educ Pract. 2018; 9: 631–638. doi: 10.2147 / AMEP.S162350.

Article   Google Scholar  

Makino K, Ikegaya Y. Learning Paradigms for the Promotion of Memory, and Their Underlying Principles. Brain Nerve. 2018;70(7):821–828. doi: https://doi.org/10.11477/mf.1416201083 .

Haile YG, Alemu SM, Habtewold TD. Insomnia and Its Temporal Association with Academic Performance among University Students: A Cross-Sectional Study. Biomed Res Int. 2017;2017:2542367. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/2542367 .

Gianfredi V, Nucci D, Tonzani A, Amodeo R, Benvenuti AL, Villarini M, Moretti M. Sleep disorder, Mediterranean Diet and learning performance among nursing students: inSOMNIA, a cross-sectional study. Ann Ig. 2018; 30(6):470–481. doi: https://doi.org/10.7416/ai.2018.2247 .

Zhao K, Zhang J, Wu Z, Shen X, Tong S, Li S. The relationship between insomnia symptoms and school performance among 4966 adolescents in Shanghai, China. Sleep Health. 2019;5(3):273–279. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.12.008 .

Alotaibi AD, Alosaimi FM, Alajlan AA, Bin Abdulrahman KA. The relationship between sleep quality, stress, and academic performance among medical students. J Family Community Med. 2020;27(1):23–28. doi: https://doi.org/10.4103/jfcm.JFCM_132_19 .

Adan, A.; Almirall, H. Horne & Östberg Morningnees-Eveningnees Questionnaire: a reduced scale. Pers Individ Dif. 1991, 12, 241–53. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90110-W

Randler C. German version of the reduced Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (rMEQ). Biological Rhythm Research. 2013;44(5):730–736. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09291016.2012.739930

Peach H, Gaultney JF. Charlotte Attitudes Towards Sleep (CATS) Scale: A validated measurement tool for college students. J Am Coll Health. 2017;65(1):22–31. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2016.1231688 .

Al-Kandari S, Alsalem A, Al-Mutairi S, Al-Lumai D, Dawoud A, Moussa M. Association between sleep hygiene awareness and practice with sleep quality among Kuwait Zhao University students. Sleep Health. 2017;3(5):342–347. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2017.06.004 .

Miró E, Iáñez MA, Cano-Lozano MC. Sleep and health patterns. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2002;2:301–326.

Google Scholar  

Zohal MA, Yazdi Z, Kazemifar AM, Mahjoob P, Ziaeeha M. Sleep Quality and Quality of Life in COPD Patients with and without Suspected Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Sleep Disord. 2014;2014:508372. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/508372.21

Núñez P, Perillan C, Arguelles J, Diaz E. Comparison of sleep and chronotype between senior and undergraduate university students. Chronobiol Int. 2019;36(12):1626–1637. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2019.1660359 .

Phillips AJK, Clerx WM, O’Brien CS, Sano A, Barger LK, Picard RW, Lockley SW, Klerman EB, Czeisler CA. Irregular sleep/wake patterns are associated with poorer academic performance and delayed circadian and sleep/wake timing. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):3216. doi: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-03171-4 .

Niño García JA, Barragán Vergel MF, Ortiz Labrador JA, Ochoa Vera ME, González Olaya HL. Factors Associated with Excessive Daytime Sleepiness in Medical Students of a Higher Education Institution of Bucaramanga. Rev Colomb Psiquiatr. 2019;48(4):222–231. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rcp.2017.12.002 .

Shochat T, Cohen-Zion M, Tzischinsky O. Functional consequences of inadequate sleep in adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2014;18:75–87. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2013.03.005

Yang G, Lai CS, Cichon J, Ma L, Li W, Gan WB. Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science. 2014;344(6188):1173–8. doi: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1249098 .

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Bruin EJ, van Run C, Staaks J, Meijer AM. Effects of sleep manipulation on cognitive functioning in adolescents: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2017; 32: 45–57. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2016.02.006 .

Arbabi T, Vollmer C, Dörfler T, Randler C The influence of timing and intelligence on academic performance in elementary school is mediated by awareness, sleep midpoint and motivation. Chronobiol Int. 2015;32(3):349–57. doi: https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2014.980508

Menon B, Karishma HP, Mamatha IV. Sleep quality and health complaints among nursing students. Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2015;18(3):363–4. doi: https://doi.org/10.4103/0972-2327.157252 .

Simonelli-Muñoz AJ, Balanza S, Rivera-Caravaca JM, Vera-Catalán T, Lorente AM, Gallego-Gómez JI. Reliability and validity of the student stress inventory-stress manifestations questionnaire and its association with personal and academic factors in university students. Nurse Educ Today. 2018;64:156–160. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2018.02.019 .

Begdache L, Kianmehr H, Sabounchi N, Marszalek A, Dolma N. Principal component regression of academic performance, substance use and sleep quality in relation to risk of anxiety and depression in young adults. Trends Neurosci Educ. 2019;15:29–37. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2019.03.002 .

LeBourgeois MK, Hale L, Chang AM, Akacem LD, Montgomery-Downs HE, Buxton OM. Digital Media and Sleep in Childhood and Adolescence. Pediatrics. 2017;140(Suppl 2):S92-S96. doi: https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758J .

Talero-Gutiérrez C, Durán-Torres F, Pérez-Olmos I. Sleep: general characteristics Physiological and pathophysiological patterns in adolescence. Revista Ciencias de la Salud. 2013;11(3):333–348.

Crowley SJ, Wolfson AR, Tarokh L, Carskadon MA. An update on adolescent sleep: New evidence informing the perfect storm model. J Adolesc. 2018;67:55–65. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.06.001 .

Suardiaz-Muro M, Morante-Ruiz M, Ortega-Moreno M, Ruiz MA, Martín-Plasencia P, Vela-Bueno A. Sleep and academic performance in university students: a systematic review. Rev Neurol. 2020;71(2):43–53. doi: https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.7102.2020015 .

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Faculty of Health Sciences, Catholic University of Murcia, 30107, Murcia, Spain

Juana Inés Gallego-Gómez, María Teresa Rodríguez González-Moro, Tomás Vera-Catalán & Serafín Balanza

Department of Pneumology, Alcalá de Henares, Hospital Universitario Príncipe de Asturias, 28805, Madrid, Spain

José Miguel Rodríguez González-Moro

Department of Nursing, Physiotherapy and Medicine, Faculty of Health Sciences,, University of Almería, Ctra. Sacramento, s/n 04120 La Cañada de San Urbano, 04007, Almería, Spain

Agustín Javier Simonelli-Muñoz

Department of Cardiology, Hospital Clínico Universitario Virgen de la Arrixaca, Universidad de Murcia, Instituto Murciano de Investigación Biosanitaria (IMIB-Arrixaca), CIBERCV, 30120, Murcia, Spain

José Miguel Rivera-Caravaca

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

JIGG, AJSM, MTRGM, TVC, and JMRGM conceptualized and designed the current study, and were major contributors in the data collection, and reviewing of the manuscript. JIGG and AJSM performed data curation, formal analysis, data interpretation, and writing of the original draft manuscript. JMRC and SB were major contributors in the writing and statistical analysis. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Agustín Javier Simonelli-Muñoz .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

The Research Ethics Committee of the Catholic University of Murcia, Spain, approved the current study (Reference: CE-6191). Along with the questionnaire, the researchers provided a letter stating the purpose and methods of the study, the voluntary nature of participation, and the confidentiality of responses. Participants signed an informed consent form.

Consent for publication

Competing interests.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1:, rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ . The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Gallego-Gómez, J.I., González-Moro, M.T.R., González-Moro, J.M.R. et al. Relationship between sleep habits and academic performance in university Nursing students. BMC Nurs 20 , 100 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12912-021-00635-x

Download citation

Received : 28 February 2021

Accepted : 10 June 2021

Published : 17 June 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12912-021-00635-x

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Sleep habits
  • Circadian rhythm
  • Sleep pattern
  • Nursing students

BMC Nursing

ISSN: 1472-6955

research paper about sleeping habits

An official website of the United States government

Official websites use .gov A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS A lock ( Lock Locked padlock icon ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

NIHPA Author Manuscripts logo

The Role of Sleep Hygiene in Promoting Public Health: A Review of Empirical Evidence

Leah a irish, christopher e kline, heather e gunn, daniel j buysse, martica h hall.

  • Author information
  • Article notes
  • Copyright and License information

Corresponding Author: Dr. Leah Irish, NDSU Dept. 2765, P.O. Box 6050, Fargo, ND 58108-6050, USA; [email protected] ; Phone: (701) 231-6242; Fax: (701) 231-8426

Issue date 2015 Aug.

The ineffectiveness of sleep hygiene as a treatment in clinical sleep medicine has raised some interesting questions. If it is known that, individually, each specific component of sleep hygiene is related to sleep, why wouldn't addressing multiple individual components (i.e., sleep hygiene education) result in improved sleep? Is there still a use for sleep hygiene? Global public health concern over poor sleep has increased the demand for effective sleep promotion strategies that are easily accessible to the general population. However, the extent to which sleep hygiene principles and strategies apply outside of clinical settings is not well known. The present review sought to evaluate the empirical evidence for several common sleep hygiene recommendations, including regular exercise, stress management, noise reduction, sleep timing regularity, and avoidance of caffeine, nicotine, alcohol, and daytime napping, with a particular emphasis on their public health utility. Thus, our review is not intended to be exhaustive regarding the clinical application of these techniques, but rather to focus on broader applications. Overall, though epidemiologic and experimental research generally supported an association between individual sleep hygiene recommendations and nocturnal sleep, the direct effects of individual recommendations on sleep remains largely untested in the general population. Suggestions for further clarification of sleep hygiene recommendations and considerations for the use of sleep hygiene in nonclinical populations are discussed.

Keywords: sleep hygiene, public health, caffeine, nicotine, alcohol, exercise, stress, noise, sleep timing, napping

Sleep hygiene is defined as a set of behavioral and environmental recommendations intended to promote healthy sleep, and was originally developed for use in the treatment of mild to moderate insomnia. 1 During sleep hygiene education, patients learn about healthy sleep habits and are encouraged to follow a set of recommendations to improve their sleep (e.g., avoid caffeine, exercise regularly, eliminate noise from the sleeping environment, maintain a regular sleep schedule). 2 Although research has demonstrated links between individual sleep hygiene components and subsequent sleep, evidence for the efficacy of sleep hygiene education as a treatment for insomnia has been limited and inconclusive. 2 - 5 Taken together, the lack of supportive data and the availability of effective, empirically supported, behavioral treatment alternatives has led to the conclusion that sleep hygiene education is ineffective as a monotherapy for insomnia. 6 Thus, we turn our attention away from sleep hygiene in the context of clinical sleep medicine, and consider its potential utility in the realm of public health where sleep hygiene is still widely used.

Sleep problems are prevalent in the global population. Throughout this manuscript, the term “sleep problems” will be used to refer generally to any combination of acute or chronic problems with prolonged sleep onset latency (SOL), excessive wake after sleep onset (WASO), short total sleep time (TST), low sleep efficiency (SE), or poor sleep quality based on subjective and/or objective assessments. We specifically do not use “sleep problems” to refer to such difficulties as symptoms of more specific clinical sleep disorders. Recent estimates suggest that over half (56%) of Americans suffered from sleep problems over the previous year, as compared to 31% of Western Europeans and 29% of Japanese. 7 Though the majority of these individuals reported functional impairment as a result of their sleep problems, most (61-79%) did not meet clinical diagnostic criteria for insomnia based on self-reported symptoms. 7 In a similar survey of adults representing 10 countries, 31.6% of participants were classified as having insomnia while an additional 17.5% of participants were classified with subthreshold insomnia. 8 Sleep problems are of growing concern to global public health because poor sleep is associated with impairments in motivation, emotion, and cognitive functioning as well as increased risk for serious medical conditions (e.g., diabetes, cardiovascular disease, cancer) and all-cause mortality, even when the symptoms are below the threshold for clinical sleep disorders. 9 - 11 Despite the potential widespread benefit for sleep promotion, established behavioral treatments (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia (CBT-I)) are largely limited to individuals who qualify for, and seek, treatment from sleep medicine professionals. The present review considers the needs of the general nonclinical population, encompassing individuals with intermittent or subsyndromal sleep impairments as well as those who may meet criteria for sleep disorders, but for whom sleep treatments are unavailable or inaccessible. Such individuals may be more likely to seek assistance from primary care providers or self-help materials to manage their sleep problems, and as a result will likely be exposed to sleep hygiene recommendations which are widely used in medical settings 2 , 3 and are easily accessible on the internet. As described recently, 12 greater emphasis on sleep health (rather than clinical sleep disorders) more closely aligns sleep research with current healthcare objectives and broadens our understanding of sleep's full spectrum of influence on population health.

Though the utility of sleep hygiene education may be limited in clinical settings, there are several reasons to consider its potential to improve sleep and promote health in the general population. In addition to being commonly used and readily available, sleep hygiene education does not require the direct involvement of a clinician and therefore can be widely disseminated to individuals not likely to seek medical treatment for their sleep problems. As a relatively inexpensive lifestyle intervention, sleep hygiene education could serve as a first-line intervention in a stepped-care model for adults who want to improve their sleep but are not likely to qualify for, or seek, more substantial clinical treatment. Sleep hygiene recommendations may be delivered via a variety of media (e.g., print- or internet-based), resulting in increased access. 13 In addition, sleep hygiene education may be a more appealing and intuitive option for the general population. For example, examination of a tailored sleep improvement plan in cancer patients revealed that when developing their individual plan, participants preferred sleep hygiene strategies over stimulus control or sleep restriction. 14 Moreover, adherence to the sleep hygiene component was relatively high and increased over time (68-78%) compared to the other treatment components. 15 However, it is important to note that individuals with undiagnosed or untreated sleep disorders may engage in poor sleep hygiene behaviors in an attempt to cope with their poor sleep (e.g., caffeine or alcohol use), and continued efforts should be made to identify these individuals and refer them for more appropriate treatments.

Recent public health campaigns have advanced general knowledge about the importance of good sleep, though they are often focused on adequate sleep duration rather than good sleep quality, and the effectiveness of these campaigns is generally unclear. Less is known regarding scientifically valid strategies by which the average person might effectively improve their sleep. Relatively few studies have investigated the efficacy of sleep hygiene interventions in nonclinical samples. 16 - 20 Overall, this work has provided some preliminary support for the use of sleep hygiene education in nonclinical populations, but the findings are inconsistent. Taken together with findings in clinical samples, these data raise an interesting question. If it is known that, individually, each specific component of sleep hygiene is related to sleep, why wouldn't addressing multiple individual components (i.e., sleep hygiene education) result in improved sleep? Inconsistent and uncompelling findings may be due, in large part, to the lack of a standardized approach in the application of sleep hygiene principles to clinical practice and research. As reviewed by Stepanski and Wyatt, 3 definitions of sleep hygiene are inconsistent across studies, and the individual recommendations vary widely in both content and implementation. Further, these authors recommended that future research focus on establishing clear guidelines for individual behavioral and environmental aspects of sleep hygiene, rather than focusing on sleep hygiene as a comprehensive list. 3 This approach is consistent with Hauri's original recommendations to tailor sleep hygiene recommendations to fit individual needs, 1 but is inconsistent with the common public health approach of providing a standard and comprehensive set of recommendations. An important next step is to consider the empirical foundation for sleep hygiene, and identify appropriate modifications to improve its delivery and efficacy in the general population.

More specifically, the current evidence base for each individual sleep hygiene recommendation should be evaluated and expanded to support further clarification of recommendations. With a particular focus on application in nonclinical populations, the present review aims to: 1) critically review the empirical evidence for individual components of sleep hygiene recommendations, identifying inconsistencies and clarifying specific guidelines for optimal sleep promotion; 2) identify gaps in the present understanding of sleep hygiene recommendations and provides suggestions for future research; and 3) identify additional conceptual and methodological issues to consider when utilizing sleep hygiene recommendations in the general population. Particular emphasis was placed on reviewing research that directly manipulated the recommended behavior by either examining the impact of the behavior or environmental factor on sleep by manipulating it (e.g., administering caffeine to a caffeine-naïve individual and observing effects on subsequent sleep) or by observing changes in sleep after the recommended behavior change was made (e.g., asking habitual caffeine users to abstain from caffeine and observing the effects on subsequent sleep). The former strategy allows for a “clean” examination of individual effects while the latter may be confounded by conceptual “noise” (e.g., tolerance, addiction, concurrent risk factors) but more closely approximates realistic circumstances in which individuals may be using sleep hygiene strategies. To maximize its relevance to the general population, when possible, the present review is focused on adults who were not specifically recruited because they suffered from clinically diagnosed sleep disorders.

Empirical support for individual sleep hygiene recommendations

Below we provide a review of the empirical support for several of the most common sleep hygiene recommendations including caffeine consumption, smoking, alcohol use, exercise, stress, noise, sleep timing, and daytime napping. The present review is not exhaustive, but reports on representative studies, with a particular emphasis on research that examined specific parameters of recommendations (e.g., timing of behavior, amount of exposure) and research that directly evaluated the change in sleep after following sleep hygiene recommendations. As noted above, emphasis was placed on studies that 1) used nonclinical samples (i.e., did not study individuals selected for insomnia), 2) tested the isolated impact of an individual sleep hygiene component on sleep (rather than in combination with other treatments or sleep hygiene components), and 3) utilized experimental designs. Research published in languages other than English was not included. Key findings are summarized in Table 1 .

Summary of key findings and future directions for the application of sleep hygiene to the general population

Note. EEG=electroencephalography; ICU=Intensive Care Unit; PSG=polysomnography; SOL=sleep onset latency.

Caffeine is the most widely used psychoactive substance in the world, 21 and its stimulant properties make it a logical target for sleep promotion efforts in the general population. On a molecular level, caffeine's alerting and sleep-disruptive effects are driven by blockade of adenosine receptors in the basal forebrain and hypothalamus (see reviews of caffeine's pharmacology 22 , 23 ). Plasma levels of caffeine peak approximately 30 minutes after oral administration, and the half-life of a single dose of caffeine is 3-7 hours, though this is influenced by individual differences in sensitivity, metabolism, and accumulation. 22 , 23 For example, the half-life of caffeine has been shown to increase with age, such that the substance remains active for longer in older adults. 24 Caffeine's impact on sleep-wake physiology is well documented, but translation of its effects into clinically relevant behavioral recommendations remains less well tested.

To establish a practical, evidence-based behavioral recommendation regarding caffeine use, it is essential to consider both the timing and amount of caffeine consumption in daily life. Current sleep hygiene recommendations vary widely, ranging from complete abstinence to avoiding caffeine only in the afternoon or evening. 3 However, few studies have actually tested the impact of morning caffeine consumption on subsequent nighttime sleep. Landolt and colleagues administered 200 mg of caffeine early in the morning and examined its effect on polysomnographic (PSG) sleep characteristics in 9 healthy, young, male participants. 25 Though salivary caffeine levels were low by bedtime, results indicated reduced TST and SE, in conjunction with a shift from lower to higher electroencephalographic (EEG) frequencies during whole-night sleep, following morning caffeine compared to placebo. Despite these intriguing results, no study has confirmed or contradicted Landolt et al.'s conclusions in the nearly 20 years since these findings were first published. One recent study examined caffeine consumed in the home environment (primarily in the morning), and found that plasma levels of caffeine and its metabolites at bedtime were not associated with PSG-assessed sleep in either people with insomnia or good sleepers. 26 Overall, though it is plausible that acute or habitual use of caffeine may impact physiological sleep-wake systems beyond the span of its half-life, replication is necessary to confirm this effect and justify a recommendation regarding morning caffeine abstinence for all individuals.

In contrast, several studies have investigated afternoon and evening caffeine use. A recent study of 12 healthy young adults administered 400 mg of caffeine in the late afternoon and evening (i.e., within the half-life of caffeine), and found that even doses ingested up to 6 hours before bedtime were associated with disturbances in both subjectively and objectively assessed sleep. 27 A review of several laboratory studies of bedtime caffeine administration indicates that administration of caffeine approximately 30 minutes before bedtime disrupts nightly sleep by increasing SOL and decreasing TST and SE, as well as shifting sleep architecture toward lighter sleep. 23 Notably, the amount of caffeine administered in these studies was often moderate to high (up to 600 mg, equivalent to approximately 5-6 cups of brewed coffee 28 ), as it was often intended to model insomnia in humans. 23 In addition, participants in these studies were often naïve to caffeine, and therefore may systematically differ from the general population of caffeine users. 23 Thus, it is reasonable to conclude that consuming large quantities of caffeine near bedtime (i.e., equivalent to several cups of coffee) is likely to disrupt sleep, but less is known about the clinical significance of low to moderate amounts of caffeine. In a direct comparison of the effects of 0, 100, 200 and 300 mg of caffeine administered shortly before bedtime, only those who received 300 mg of caffeine showed significant sleep impairments in comparison to those who received no caffeine. 29 A few other studies have examined low to moderate doses of self- administered caffeine. For example, Lloret-Linares and colleagues conducted a double-blind trial to compare the effects of one cup of caffeinated vs. decaffeinated coffee after dinner on self-reported sleep in individuals who identified themselves as caffeine-sensitive. 30 Results indicated several significant effects of caffeinated coffee on self-reported sleep quality, but these findings may not generalize to the habitually caffeinated population. In fact, recent work has identified an adenosine receptor gene associated with caffeine sensitivity, and reported that the impact of caffeine on the sleep of caffeine-insensitive individuals may be minimal. 31 Thus, individuals might consider their own caffeine sensitivity before changing caffeine intake as a means to improve sleep. Another recent randomized, double-blind study compared 5 days of placebo to 5 days of caffeine (250 mg) self-administered 0-60 minutes before bedtime, and measured effects on self-reported and actigraphy-assessed sleep. 32 On the first night, there was significantly greater sleep fragmentation, poorer self-reported sleep quality, and a trend toward lower SE for those in the caffeine condition relative to placebo. Beyond the first night, however, only actigraphy-assessed SE was significantly lower in the caffeine condition. The authors interpreted these changes in between-group differences over the course of just a few days as indicative of tolerance to caffeine's effects. 32 Similarly rapid tolerance to the acute sleep-disrupting effects of caffeine administration has been observed in several other laboratory studies (see review 23 ), yet the role of tolerance in attenuating caffeine's influence on sleep in habitual caffeine consumers remains largely unexplored. Finally, Hindmarch and colleagues 34 utilized a cross-over design to test the impact of tea (37.5 or 75 mg of caffeine) and coffee (75 or 150 mg of caffeine) consumption on nocturnal sleep in 30 habitual caffeine users. Participants received one type of caffeinated beverage (or water) at 0900, 1300, 1700, and 2100 hours for one day with a 6day washout period between beverage conditions. Results indicated that, compared to water, caffeine consumption was associated with greater self-reported difficulty falling asleep and lower sleep quality after controlling for the previous night's sleep, though these effects were notable only for the highest caffeine condition (coffee with 150 mg caffeine). Significant effects were also observed for actigraphy-assessed TST. In comparison to no caffeine (water), low-dose caffeine conditions (tea with 37.5 or 75 mg caffeine, coffee with 75 mg caffeine) resulted in approximately 15 fewer minutes of actigraphic TST and high-dose caffeine (coffee with 150 mg caffeine) resulted in almost 45 fewer minutes of actigraphic TST. Interestingly, this reduction in TST was moderated by habitual caffeine use, such that individuals with a lower habitual caffeine intake were more sensitive to the sleep-disrupting effects of caffeine than individuals with a higher habitual caffeine intake. 34 These data further support consideration of issues of caffeine tolerance and sensitivity in research targeting samples of habitual users.

Although many studies have examined the impact of caffeine administration on subsequent sleep, very few have examined the impact of caffeine avoidance by habitual users, despite the fact that this population would be most likely to receive, and benefit from, such recommendations. Of the few available studies, the results have been inconsistent. 34 - 36 Hofer and Battig 35 assigned habitual caffeine users to caffeine, no caffeine, or intermittent caffeine conditions. Results suggested slightly fewer problems falling asleep on days without caffeine, but found no other effects of caffeine abstinence on self-reported sleep characteristics. Similarly, James 36 assigned habitual caffeine users to one of four groups designed to represent caffeine abstinence, acute use, withdrawal, or habitual use. After one week, the withdrawal group (6 days caffeine, 1 day placebo) reported longer sleep duration compared to the mean of the other 3 groups. In a recent study, Ho and colleagues 34 assigned habitual users to one week of caffeine or abstinence and found no change from baseline self-reported or actigraphy-assessed sleep. These null or modest effects may indicate that caffeine cessation by habitual users is not very effective at improving sleep, perhaps because tolerance has developed to caffeine's sleep-disrupting effects. However, it is important to note that participants of these studies were not screened for sleep problems prior to recruitment, often because the impact of caffeine on sleep was not a primary aim of the study. The limited effects may have been observed because there was little room for improvement in sleep from baseline. This also suggests that these samples are not representative of the individuals most likely to use sleep hygiene recommendations (i.e., people with sleep problems). Additional limitations include small sample size, 34 restrictive sources of caffeine (i.e., coffee only, 34 , 35 caffeine pill 36 ), and reliance on self-reported sleep. 35 , 36 Thus, at present it is difficult to interpret the implications of these findings, but future work might extend this research by investigating the short- and long-term impact of caffeine cessation in habitual users with sleep problems.

In sum, laboratory studies have demonstrated that large doses of caffeine close to bedtime have an acute disruptive effect on human sleep, but the effects of lower doses of caffeine are smaller in magnitude and less consistent. The long-term effects of caffeine abstinence in habitual caffeine consumers are not yet known, but the limited evidence to date suggests caffeine abstinence may be more beneficial for light or intermittent caffeine users than for habitual users. In order to form effective behavioral recommendations on caffeine use and sleep, future research might consider several key points. Self-administration studies in the home environment would more closely model typical use of caffeine in the general population. Careful measurement and/or manipulation of realistic timing and dosage of caffeine intake would clarify recommendations regarding caffeine dose and timing. Finally, additional research should consider the role of caffeine tolerance and its implications for sleep. For low to moderate habitual users, the daily deviation from normal caffeine intake may be more relevant to sleep than absolute amounts. Thus, behavioral recommendations may be personalized based on an individual's habits, rather than a generalized rule. Advancements in the study of caffeine and sleep will help identify the strength of their association in regular users, and inform the establishment of practical behavioral recommendations.

Like caffeine, nicotine promotes arousal and wakefulness, primarily through stimulation of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain. 22 Sleep hygiene recommendations suggest avoidance of nicotine use to promote better sleep. As reviewed by Jaehne and colleagues, nicotine, whether from cigarette smoking or administration via pill or patch, is associated with impaired sleep. 37 Specifically, nicotine use is associated with increased SOL, decreased TST, more frequent early morning awakening and suppression of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and slow-wave sleep (SWS), though these findings are not entirely consistent across self-report and PSG assessments (see 37 ). Thus, the recommendation to avoid and/or discontinue use of nicotine to improve sleep seems reasonable. However, when recommending that a nicotine-dependent individual abstain to promote better sleep, it is critical to evaluate the direct effects of smoking cessation on sleep.

In the early stages of smoking cessation, sleep complaints are very common. This is likely due to nicotine withdrawal, which results in heightened arousal and cravings. Symptoms of withdrawal peak a few days after cessation and last for 3-4 weeks. 38 Increases in the frequency and duration of arousals are the most consistent sleep problems during withdrawal, as assessed by both self-report and PSG, and have been shown to increase risk of relapse in the weeks following cessation (see 37 , 39 ). Although the acute negative impact of smoking cessation on sleep is well documented, the long-term impact of smoking cessation on sleep, and therefore, whether it is a viable sleep hygiene recommendation, is not well known. Two large cohort studies compared the sleep of non-smokers, former smokers, and current smokers and found that former smokers did not differ significantly from non-smokers in either self-reported 40 or PSG-assessed 41 sleep. These results suggest that sleep impairments associated with smoking may be resolved after cessation and long-term sleep improvements can be achieved following smoking cessation, but such conclusions are limited by the study methodologies. Both studies defined former smokers as individuals who have smoked but do not currently smoke, and additional data were not available regarding important characteristics such as the degree of past exposure and time elapsed since cessation. Moreover, sleep data prior to smoking cessation were not available for comparison in former smokers. Additional research that examines the longitudinal trajectory of sleep from pre-cessation through withdrawal and long-term assessments would help clarify the benefits of this sleep hygiene recommendation.

The current literature also revealed several additional aspects to consider in constructing a useful and valid recommendation to improve sleep with regard to nicotine use. Although general physiological tolerance to nicotine develops quickly 22 this does not appear to translate into tolerance for nicotine's sleep-disrupting effects. Whereas caffeine tolerance results in a lesser impact on sleep disturbance, data suggest that even after years of smoking, smokers experience significantly worse sleep than nonsmokers. 37 Cigarette smoke contains many chemicals other than nicotine, and it may be that other agents disrupt sleep without developing tolerance. Alternatively, the discrepancy between smokers and nonsmokers may reflect other systematic differences between these self-selected groups. Very few studies 41 - 42 have adjusted for relevant group differences such as age, gender, medication use, sleep apnea, and other health risk behaviors (e.g., alcohol and caffeine use, physical inactivity) known to influence sleep, so it is difficult to draw broad conclusions about the independent effect of nicotine on sleep among smokers compared to non-smokers.

Research documenting the effects of nicotine on sleep in occasional smokers or passive (secondhand) smokers is also limited. For example, Gillin and colleagues conducted a double-blind cross-over trial to examine the impact of nicotine patch administration on PSG-assessed sleep in 12 healthy nonsmokers. 43 The amount of nicotine received was equivalent to several cigarettes. 22 Compared to placebo, nicotine administration resulted in REM suppression and early awakening, with REM rebound and normalization of wake time occurring during the subsequent recovery night. Altogether, the few studies on this topic suggest that nicotine use in nondependent smokers has acute consequences similar to use in dependent smokers. Based on current data, it is not yet clear if complete abstention from nicotine is required for optimal sleep, or if there is a dose-response relationship or a threshold under which sleep would not be disturbed (e.g., after one cigarette). This information could be relevant to individuals’ self-efficacy for following sleep hygiene recommendations and their willingness to adhere. Passive smoke is another way in which nonsmokers may be exposed to nicotine. Surveys suggest that secondhand smoke exposure may be linked to sleep disturbance, 44 , 45 though one survey defined sleep disturbance broadly as reporting insufficient sleep or rest at least one day during the past month. 45 Davila and colleagues defined sleep problems more stringently using 10 questions about diagnostic criteria for sleep disorders, and found no differences in sleep between nonsmokers with and without secondhand smoke exposure. 46

In conclusion, evidence suggests that exposure to nicotine is associated with sleep problems, particularly at high doses. Recommendations to discontinue nicotine use, however, are complicated by the temporary worsening of sleep in the acute withdrawal period following cessation and the limited evidence regarding long-term benefits. Though it seems plausible that recommendations to avoid occasional and passive smoking would also be beneficial, data are limited at this time. Future systematic evaluation of the long-term impact of smoking cessation on sleep will inform behavioral recommendations, and the incorporation of strategies to help smokers overcome withdrawal-related sleep disturbance may further improve the efficacy of sleep hygiene recommendations regarding nicotine use. In addition, further investigation of occasional and passive smoking will clarify accurate recommendations for non-dependent smokers by evaluating factors such as timing, frequency, and type of nicotine exposure. Current sleep hygiene recommendations regarding nicotine use are not likely to be generalizable to all individuals, and therefore efforts should be made to define guidelines appropriate for individual circumstances.

Alcohol use is another behavior commonly discouraged in sleep hygiene education, with recommendations ranging from complete abstinence to avoidance of excessive use just before bedtime. 3 The acute effects of alcohol administration on sleep in healthy individuals are reasonably consistent and well documented. Alcohol administration near bedtime is associated with decreased SOL and increased SWS during the first part of the night. However, once the alcohol is metabolized within the first few hours of sleep, subsequent sleep becomes lighter with increases in Stage 1 and REM sleep and more arousals (see 47 - 49 for reviews). These effects result from alcohol's influence on several neurochemical systems (e.g., GABA, adenosine; see 47 , 48 ).

Despite the compelling body of work demonstrating the impact of alcohol administration on sleep, fewer studies have examined the effects of alcohol reduction on subsequent sleep that would be expected if individuals were to follow the sleep hygiene recommendation. Upon completing a 1-day sleep hygiene education program designed to eliminate alcohol intake at bedtime, Morita and colleagues found a reduction in daytime sleepiness that coincided with reduction in alcohol use at bedtime. 19 However, it is unclear whether changes in sleep should be attributed to changes in alcohol use because data were not presented separately for alcohol users versus nonusers. 19 For dependent users, long-term alcohol abstinence may result in modest sleep improvement. 51 , 52 However, even after years of abstinence, many sleep problems may persist, including shorter sleep duration, lighter sleep, and greater sleep fragmentation (see 53 ). In addition, research investigating sleep and alcohol cessation in dependent users often focuses on the acute withdrawal stage. As with caffeine, tolerance to alcohol's sleep-disrupting effects occurs within days, and sleep parameters return to baseline for healthy nondependent individuals, even at high doses of alcohol administration. 47 , 48 In contrast, alcohol-dependent individuals suffer from chronic sleep disturbance, which may suggest that they do not habituate to alcohol's effects on sleep or that the dosage and timing of chronic users may elicit systematically different effects than in light or non-drinkers. Chronic alcohol use may result in lasting alterations to key physiological systems which contribute to sleep regulation. 54 Further, acute nighttime withdrawal symptoms may perpetuate sleep problems resulting from acute use, 55 which can exacerbate other alcohol-related withdrawal symptoms and ultimately increase risk for alcohol relapse. 48

Translation of alcohol's influence on sleep into effective sleep-promoting recommendations is somewhat complicated and requires additional attention to several key factors, such as alcohol amount and timing. To date, most evidence is based on alcohol administration within 60 minutes of bedtime, and few studies have examined the effect of late afternoon or early evening drinking on nocturnal sleep, although there are several circumstances in which this type of drinking may occur (e.g., consumption of alcohol during “happy hour” immediately after work or during afternoon and evening meals). Landolt and colleagues administered alcohol to 10 middle-aged men 6 hours before bedtime. 56 Although breath alcohol levels had reached zero by bedtime, the reported effects on PSG-assessed sleep were similar to those found with bedtime administration. These findings were consistent with an earlier study in 4 healthy participants, 57 but have not yet been replicated, and plausible mechanisms to explain alcohol's effect on sleep following its metabolism have not yet been clearly identified and tested.

In addition to timing, the alcohol amounts should be considered when defining behavioral guidelines. Studies in healthy adults have generally shown a dose-response relationship between the amount of alcohol consumed and sleep onset and depth, suggesting that higher doses of alcohol are associated with worse sleep (see 47 - 49 ). Effects are typically smaller and less consistent at lower doses of alcohol, which suggests that occasional and light consumption (1-3 standard drinks 49 ) may be less likely to disrupt sleep than moderate or heavy doses. Recommendations regarding absolute amounts of alcohol, however, should consider the gender of the individual as data suggest women's blood alcohol levels are significantly higher than men's after consuming the same amount and type of alcohol. 58

In sum, for non-dependent individuals, occasional consumption of alcohol (even light amounts) shortly before bedtime can impair sleep that night. The impact of afternoon or early evening alcohol use on sleep is not yet clearly understood. For alcohol-dependent individuals, chronic sleep problems are common, which are exacerbated during acute alcohol withdrawal. Thus, the effectiveness of this sleep hygiene recommendation for alcohol-dependent individuals is not well known. Additional research should consider alcohol cessation and sleep for a variety of naturalistic alcohol use patterns (e.g., dependent users, habitual weekend drinkers, daily glass of wine with dinner) to more clearly identify use patterns that increase risk for sleep problems and should be targeted in sleep hygiene recommendations. Also, future research should investigate the collective effect of alcohol use with other substances and more carefully explore their combined impact on sleep. For example, alcohol and cigarettes are often used simultaneously, and some alcoholic beverages may be combined with caffeinated beverages (e.g., coffee, soda, energy drinks), 59 making their effects on sleep difficult to predict. These aspects of alcohol use warrant further investigation in order to provide behavioral recommendations that are meaningful to a broader audience.

Regular exercise is a common sleep hygiene recommendation, with the caveat that exercise should be avoided close to bedtime. 3 Although its mechanisms are largely unknown, exercise may improve sleep through its effects on body temperature, arousal, and/or adenosine levels. 60 Despite recommendation regarding the sleep-enhancing benefits of habitual exercise, much of the evidence is based on examination of the impact of acute bouts of exercise on a subsequent night's sleep. Two meta-analyses found that acute exercise produces modest increases in PSG-assessed TST, NREM stage 2 sleep, SWS, and latency to REM sleep, as well as a small reduction in SOL, 61 , 62 though these findings are somewhat limited due to the reliance upon young adult participants without sleep disturbances. 62 To date, only one study has examined the effects of acute exercise on sleep in adults with insomnia; in this study, an acute bout of moderate-intensity aerobic exercise performed in the late afternoon substantially improved PSG- and diary-assessed SOL and TST on the subsequent night, whereas neither high-intensity aerobic exercise nor moderate- or high-intensity resistance exercise altered sleep compared to a baseline night. 63 Taken together, this evidence suggests that an acute bout of exercise is likely to result in a modest improvement in the subsequent night's sleep, but this claim should be confirmed in more representative samples of individuals with nonclinical sleep complaints.

Other studies have focused on the impact of exercise training (i.e., ≥ 4 weeks of exercise at a specific weekly dose) on sleep in various populations with sleep disturbance, most prominently in older adults. Across these studies, a moderate-sized improvement in subjective sleep quality following exercise training is the most consistent finding; 64 the few studies that assessed sleep with PSG have produced equivocal findings. Less is known regarding the effect of exercise training on sleep in healthy individuals, though a meta-analysis suggested that the effects of chronic exercise on PSG-assessed sleep in young adults without sleep disturbances were similar to those observed for acute exercise (e.g., increased TST and SWS, decreased SOL). 61 Thus, for those with and without sleep complaints, exercise training is associated with modest improvements in sleep.

Evidence-based recommendations regarding exercise are difficult to substantiate due to its numerous components (e.g., duration, mode, intensity, timing), as well as whether an acute exercise bout or an exercise training regimen is being considered. From earlier meta-analyses, duration of exercise moderated the acute effects of exercise on TST; the largest increases in TST were seen with exercise longer than 60 minutes. The mode of exercise performed (e.g., walking, resistance exercise) and participant's level of fitness generally were not significant moderators. 61 , 62 The extent to which these findings may differ in individuals with subclinical sleep problems is not clear. Training studies have typically employed moderate-intensity aerobic exercise and/or moderate-intensity resistance exercise at doses that approximate public health guidelines; however, direct comparisons between different modes of exercise have not been performed in those with subclinical sleep problems. Direct comparisons of different doses of exercise have also been rare: Singh et al. found similar improvements in subjective sleep quality between low- and high-intensity resistance exercise in older adults, 65 whereas Kline and colleagues found a dose-response relationship between the weekly duration of moderate-intensity aerobic exercise and improvement in subjective sleep quality in postmenopausal women. 66

The timing of exercise is another important factor that may impact sleep, especially in light of the common warning that exercising too close to bedtime could increase physiological arousal and disrupt subsequent sleep. However, as reviewed by Youngstedt, it is also plausible that exercising close to bedtime may improve sleep due to the acute body-heating, anxiolytic and antidepressant effects of exercise. 60 The effects of exercise on core body temperature may be especially important during the afternoon or evening, as sleep onset typically coincides with the rapid decline in body temperature 67 and exercise increases the rate of decline in body temperature by initially raising core body temperature. 68 According to a meta-analysis, exercising 4-8 h prior to bedtime has the most robust effects on subsequent sleep compared to all other times of day, including decreased PSG-assessed SOL and WASO. 62 However, studies have also found that exercising within 4 hours of bedtime does not disrupt 69 or even improves subsequent sleep. 70 These results were achieved even though exercise induced increases in heart rate and core body temperature that, in some cases, remained elevated at bedtime. 69 These findings were further confirmed by a recent epidemiologic survey of 1000 adults which reported that nighttime exercise was not associated with poor sleep. 71 Thus, the available evidence does not support the claim that late-night exercise disturbs sleep in the general population.

Overall, accumulating research suggests that exercise may be a useful behavioral approach for reducing sleep disturbance. However, exercise is a complex behavior with multiple facets to consider (e.g., mode, duration, intensity, timing), and these factors have not been sufficiently examined to allow for specific behavioral recommendations regarding the impact of exercise on sleep in the general population. Future research, conducted in adults with nonclinical sleep complaints, should directly compare different modes and doses of exercise, differentiate between acute and chronic effects of exercise on sleep, and evaluate the impact of exercise timing on sleep. It will be important to explore bidirectional links between exercise and sleep, as recent data suggest that sleep may be an important predictor of physical activity participation the following day. 72 Also unknown is whether the effects of exercise on sleep are similar across genders, age groups, and differing fitness levels. These research directions will help clarify the relationship between exercise and sleep and inform evidence-based recommendations on how exercise could be optimally prescribed to improve sleep in the general population.

Although stress is not traditionally a core component of sleep hygiene, several recommendations have emerged over the years encouraging individuals to reduce worry or engage in relaxing activities, particularly right before bedtime. 3 For the purpose of the present review, the term stress refers to an event or events that lead to acute or chronic physiological (increased heart rate and blood pressure) and psychological (anxiousness, vigilance) responses. Stress can precipitate cognitive arousal (i.e., worry) and physiological arousal, which are both antithetical to problems with sleep initiation and maintenance. Indeed, numerous studies have observed an association between psychosocial stress and sleep (see 73 ). Psychological stress increases psychophysiological arousal which is thought to be a primary mechanism through which stress disrupts sleep, particularly when the arousal is present at bedtime. For example, Morin and colleagues found that cognitive pre-sleep arousal (i.e., rumination before bedtime) mediated the association between daily stressors and subjective sleep quality. 74 Further, Hall and colleagues found that exposure to acute anticipatory stress close to bedtime resulted in increased sympathetic arousal, wakefulness throughout the night and less restorative sleep as measured by PSG. 75 Thus, stress management techniques (encompassing those described in sleep hygiene recommendations) have been proposed to reduce arousal associated with psychosocial stress. Designated worry time or writing a worry list has been shown to reduce sleep complaints in some individuals, 76 but limited data are available to confirm these results, particularly in nonclinical samples.

Techniques known to reduce stress and arousal, such as relaxation and mindfulness-based stress reduction, have been examined in relation to sleep and have provided some preliminary support for stress management as an effective recommendation to promote sleep. A number of specific relaxation techniques are available, and most have been linked to improved sleep in individuals with insomnia (see brief review in 77 - 79 ). Borkovec and Fowles suggested that relaxation is not directly responsible for better sleep, but rather, relaxation is focused attention that is incompatible with cognitive arousal. 80 To that end, mindfulness, which is described as focused attention on the present moment without judgment, 81 has been utilized to reduce stress in multiple populations 82 and has been associated with improved subjective sleep quality. For example, in a small community sample, individuals who participated in an 8-week mindfulness meditation course which resulted in post-course improvements in self-reported sleep. 83 Similarly, college students who increased their mindfulness through a meditative movement course experienced a significant reduction in perceived stress, which was associated with better subjective sleep quality. 84 Mindfulness training has also successfully reduced pre-sleep arousal and worry in individuals with insomnia. 85 Overall, this work provides promising findings to support the role of stress management in improving sleep, though many of these studies relied on subjective sleep assessments. It is not yet clear whether a specific focus on stress reduction conveys any benefit to sleep above and beyond general reduction in arousal.

Perhaps moreso than with any other sleep hygiene component, attention to individual differences is important for the reduction of stress-related arousal and its subsequent effects on sleep. For many individuals, the effects of acute psychosocial stress on sleep may resolve when the stressor is resolved. 86 , 87 However, an individual's perception of stress and coping style can exacerbate or prolong stress’ impact on sleep. 74 , 88 For example, individuals who describe themselves as sensitive to stress had more arousals and more sleep stage transitions as measured by PSG. 89 In addition, concurrent psychopathology may further exacerbate stress and negatively influence sleep. Thus, individual circumstances will likely influence the utility of stress management recommendations. In the absence of clinical guidance, it is challenging to direct individuals toward the stress management technique most appropriate to their needs (e.g., constructive worry, relaxation, mindfulness). Future research efforts might consider the process by which the general population can evaluate their personal experience of stress and pre-sleep arousal and identify effective strategies to address them, thereby improving sleep.

Noise is a relatively clear source of sleep disturbance, and sleep hygiene recommendations frequently advise individuals to minimize noise in their sleeping environment. However, nocturnal noises within one's normal surroundings (e.g., local traffic, music, plumbing) have the potential to impact sleep, even if they are not consciously observed. The extant literature has employed a wide range of methodologies to evaluate the impact of noise during sleep. In general, nocturnal noise increases number of arousals and results in lighter sleep (increased Stage 1 and 2 and/or suppressed SWS and REM sleep; see 11 , 90 for reviews). Laboratory studies using PSG have reported habituation to noise exposure during sleep within a few days. 91 , 92 However, autonomic arousal during sleep and subjective sleep complaints have not been shown to habituate within the limited time window employed in these studies (i.e., a few weeks). 91 , 92 Research suggests that the relationship between noise and sleep is moderated by characteristics of the noise itself (e.g., continuity, type, relevance) and to individual differences in noise sensitivity (see 11 ). Additional research employing strict methodologies to investigate the impact of environmental noise on sleep, particularly in the natural environment, could provide useful information to help individuals identify noises that are most likely to disrupt their sleep.

Even when the source of the noise is not directly modifiable, evidence-based strategies can be used to reduce the impact of noise on sleep. Much of this work has been conducted in the context of the intensive care unit (ICU) to improve patients’ sleep. As reviewed by Xie and colleagues, both sound-reducing (i.e., ear plugs) and sound-masking (i.e., white noise) strategies have been shown to improve sleep in ICU patients. 93 Additional research has investigated the efficacy of traffic noise reduction strategies in the home environment and found modest sleep improvement, including reduced SOL, 94 increased SWS, 94 , 95 and improved subjective sleep quality. 95 , 96 Future research on the impact of noise on sleep might continue to examine noise management strategies in environments representative of the general population, and evaluate individual differences in the preference and efficacy of various sound-attenuating strategies including important effect modifiers (e.g., age). In sum, the sleep hygiene recommendation to reduce noise in the sleeping environment appears sound, and the continued development and testing of noise management strategies will provide the tools required for individuals to comply with this recommendation.

Sleep timing regularity

Sleep hygiene recommendations often encourage regular bed- and/or wake-times which are intended to maximize the synchrony between physiological sleep drive, circadian rhythms, and the nocturnal sleep episode 3 . Homeostatic sleep drive and the circadian system work together to promote stable patterns of sleep and wakefulness. 97 Sleep duration and continuity are worsened when the primary sleep episode is advanced (i.e., shifted earlier) or delayed (shifted later) from one's habitual timing of sleep, such as changes in sleep patterns associated with jet lag or rotating shift work (see 98 ). In addition, irregular bed- and wake-times increase inter-night variability in sleep timing which, in turn, results in desynchrony between sleep-wake timing and other endogenous circadian rhythms. In clinical populations, research has suggested that individuals with insomnia have more inter-night variability in sleep timing 99 , 100 and self-reported and actigraphy-assessed sleep characteristics. 100 Therefore, some sleep hygiene recommendations encourage regularity of sleep timing. On the other hand, current clinical sleep medicine therapies (i.e., stimulus control, CBT-I) call for regularity in wake time but permit variability in bedtime as individuals are instructed not to go to bed until they are sleepy. 101 These treatments are effective at improving sleep in clinical populations, but the isolated benefit of the sleep timing component is not known, and the extent to which a sleep timing recommendation is effective in the absence of a clinician's guidance is unclear.

Several studies of nonclinical adult populations have examined the association between sleep timing regularity and sleep. Though not entirely consistent, 102 these data typically suggest that irregular sleep schedules are associated with greater daytime sleepiness 103 and worse self-reported sleep quality. 104 , 105 However, these data are somewhat limited by the self-report nature of both the sleep timing and sleep characteristics, which may be similarly biased by participant recall. Additional aspects of sleep timing may influence its impact on nocturnal sleep, such as differences between weekday and weekend schedules or the relative importance of regularity in wake time as compared to bedtime. 106 Future work should aim to replicate these findings using objective verification of sleep timing and sleep characteristics. Greater lifestyle regularity has also been associated with better sleep, 107 though the specific contribution of sleep timing cannot be determined.

To date, only a few studies have directly tested the efficacy of this recommendation by assigning individuals to adopt a regular sleep schedule and observing the effects on subsequent sleep. Bonnet and Alter recruited 12 college students with irregular bed- and wake-times, and assigned them to a regular sleep schedule in a sleep laboratory for 38 consecutive nights, with time in bed remaining consistent with baseline. 108 Compared to the 2-week baseline period, the regular sleep schedule resulted in increased self-reported awakenings with no significant changes in PSG-assessed sleep. Similarly, Takasu and colleagues assigned a rigid sleep schedule to 14 college students with irregular baseline sleep timing and found no significant changes in actigraphy-assessed sleep or self-reported sleepiness and alertness after 6 days. 109 Neither study recruited participants based on presence of sleep problems which, combined with small sample sizes, could explain these null findings. Youngstedt and colleagues recruited a sample of older adults with long self-reported sleep duration to examine the effect of a 90-minute reduction of time in bed. 110 Both experimental and control groups followed a fixed sleep-wake schedule for 8 weeks and results indicated that the control group (with no reduction of time in bed) experienced a significant decrease in actigraphy-assessed SE from baseline. Again, participants had no other sleep complaints at baseline. In contrast, Manber and colleagues recruited 39 college students who reported both irregular sleep schedules and excessive daytime sleepiness. 111 Participants assigned to a 4-week regular sleep-wake schedule reported significantly decreased daytime sleepiness compared with controls. Authors also noted decreased SOL and increased SE from baseline to post-intervention, but these changes were not significantly different between groups.

In sum, the evidence demonstrates a clear association between sleep schedule irregularity and sleep problems, though the data in nonclinical samples are somewhat limited by their self-report nature. Research investigating the impact of changing from an irregular to a regular sleep schedule may not generalize to the population most likely to use sleep hygiene recommendations because the participants did not have any sleep complaints. Though untested, it is plausible that a dose-response relationship may exist between sleep timing regularity and sleep problems, which could help explain the discrepant benefits of sleep timing regularity between individuals with insomnia and individuals without sleep complaints. Future research should examine the validity of this sleep hygiene recommendation in samples likely to use it (i.e., individuals with nonclinical sleep complaints) using objective assessments of both sleep timing and sleep characteristics. In light of current clinical practices, bedtime regularity should not be used as a general recommendation and future research could examine the relative impact of fixing bed and/or wake times to clarify these effects in a nonclinical population.

Daytime napping has also been posited to disrupt the homeostatic sleep drive, and sleep hygiene recommendations often include the recommendation to avoid naps of greater than 30 minutes (see 3 ). Research on the association between daytime napping and nocturnal sleep has focused primarily on older adults. Contrary to expectations, the majority of this work has identified no significant association between daytime napping and nocturnal sleep, whether assessed by self-report, 112 , 113 actigraphy, 114 or PSG. 112 , 115 Similarly, in a group of healthy, young and middle-aged adults, Pilcher and colleagues found no significant association between daytime napping and self-reported nocturnal sleep. 116 A few exceptions do exist, reporting that, in older adults, daytime napping is associated with more self-reported sleep problems 117 and greater actigraphy-assessed WASO and fragmentation and lower SE. 113 Typically, naps in these participants were longer than 30 minutes, and these data do not support a strong relationship between daytime napping and nocturnal sleep in the general population. However, these data are not entirely consistent, warranting consideration of experimental data regarding napping and nocturnal sleep.

In addition to correlational research, several studies have directly tested the impact of napping on nocturnal sleep by introducing a daytime nap (ranging from 1 day to 1 month of napping) and observing its effects on subsequent sleep. Three such studies in samples of midlife women and older adults reported no changes in PSG-assessed nocturnal sleep following daytime napping. 118 - 120 Similarly, Campbell and colleagues reported no significant changes in PSG-assessed sleep of older adults following a daytime nap, with the exception of a small but significant increase in SOL (from 16 to 22 minutes). 121 In contrast, others have found that implementing a daytime napping schedule does negatively impact PSG-assessed sleep. Monk and colleagues compared the sleep of older adults following counterbalanced assignment to 2 weeks of afternoon napping for 90 min/day and 2 weeks of sedentary control. 122 Self-reported sleep did not differ between the two conditions, but PSG-assessed sleep following the nap condition was worse in comparison to sleep following the no-nap control. Specifically, results revealed shorter TST, lower SE, and earlier wake times following 2 weeks of daily afternoon napping. However, in light of more positive findings that objective evening sleepiness was decreased during the nap condition, the authors concluded that a 90-minute afternoon nap would have very minimal adverse effects on nocturnal sleep of healthy older adults. 122 Only one study to date has manipulated napping in healthy young adults. Werth and colleagues compared baseline PSG-assessed sleep to nocturnal sleep following one evening nap. 123 Results indicated that napping was harmful to several characteristics of nocturnal sleep including PSG-assessed TST, SE, SWS, REM latency, spectral slow wave activity, and self-reported SOL. Thus, the data directly examining napping's effect on nocturnal sleep are inconclusive. No study to date has actually examined the effects of eliminating napping in a nonclinical sample in an attempt to improve the sleep of habitual nappers (i.e., following the sleep hygiene recommendation to avoid daytime naps), and thus direct empirical support for this recommendation is presently limited.

Specific characteristics of the nap itself may be considered in further refining sleep hygiene recommendations regarding napping. The duration of the nap may influence the extent to which it interferes with endogenous sleep rhythms. Although evidence clearly demonstrates that short naps (< 30 minutes) are beneficial to cognitive performance, alertness, and mood (see 124 , 125 ), this 30 minute threshold may not apply to the effects of napping on nocturnal sleep. Indeed, many of the null findings reported above included naps of greater than 30 minutes. Pilcher and colleagues directly compared the effects of nap duration (no nap, <20 minute nap, >20 minute nap) for 7 days and found no significant associations between any nap duration and self-reported nocturnal sleep. 126 In addition, few studies have directly evaluated nap timing. Evening naps, in particular, may be problematic if they dissipate homeostatic drive, thus interfering with nocturnal sleep. Yoon and colleagues examined evening naps in postmenopausal women and found that evening nap duration was associated with earlier wake times and more daytime napping. 127 Surprisingly, this study also found that evening nappers had higher actigraphy-assessed SE and lower WASO than non-evening nappers. Another study of older adults reported similar positive findings, such that individuals who napped during the daytime and evening had lower actigraphy-assessed SOL and WASO and higher SE than individuals who napped during the daytime only. 128 In contrast to these habitual, naturalistic naps, an assigned evening nap was shown to negatively impact PSG-assessed sleep, though this study only recorded one day of napping. 123 Overall, there are insufficient data to inform modification of recommendations regarding nap timing or duration.

Future investigations of napping and nocturnal sleep would benefit from an increased emphasis on the impact of changing napping behavior on subsequent nocturnal sleep. In addition to considering nap characteristics such as duration and timing, future work should address some methodological limitations with the present literature. The majority of this work has been performed in laboratory settings with scheduled nap opportunities. Additional research in more naturalistic settings is required and further investigation should consider the effects of habitual napping. Regular napping is a routine practice for many individuals (see 124 , 125 ), but it is not yet known whether nocturnal sleep habituates to the influence of daytime napping, similar to habituation to daytime caffeine. Also, habitual nappers may have a stronger 24-hour sleep drive and therefore are able to nap without impacting nighttime sleep. It is not yet known whether occasional nappers are more vulnerable than habitual nappers to the effects of napping on nocturnal sleep. As with other sleep hygiene components, individual differences are also likely to influence the consequences of daytime napping on nocturnal sleep. In particular, age is highly relevant to natural changes in circadian rhythms as well as lifestyle and health-related factors which may influence sleep timing. 129 For example, Yoon and colleagues compared napping between young and older adults and found that older adults were more likely to nap in the evening while younger adults were more likely to nap in the afternoon. 130 Further, the relationships between naps and nocturnal sleep differed by age. Older adult evening nappers had shorter SOL, earlier wake times, and earlier circadian phase compared with older adults who did not take evening naps, whereas young adults who napped in the afternoon did not differ in nocturnal sleep from young adults who did not nap in the afternoon. 130 These findings demonstrate the importance of extension of this research into a variety of populations and contexts. In sum, only limited research is available to support the common sleep hygiene recommendation to avoid naps, which may suggest that this particular recommendation is not applicable to the nonclinical population. Additional research in a broader range of contexts and populations would be required to determine if (and under which circumstances) avoidance of naps may improve sleep in the general adult population.

Conclusions

The present review evaluated the empirical support for individual sleep hygiene recommendations for adults with nonclinical sleep problems. Specifically, we performed a selective review of research investigating the impact of caffeine use, smoking, alcohol use, exercise, stress management, noise, sleep timing, and napping on nocturnal sleep characteristics. Epidemiologic and laboratory research provide some support for the relationships between individual sleep hygiene components and sleep, and each recommendation is supported by plausible physiological and psychosocial mechanisms. However, the present review identified several critical gaps in the current evidence for the use of sleep hygiene recommendations in the general population. First, direct evaluation of the effects of following sleep hygiene recommendations is scant and inconclusive for many individual recommendations. Such data are necessary to validate the extrapolation from sleep disruption studies (e.g., does administering caffeine result in impaired sleep?) to sleep hygiene recommendations (e.g., does abstaining from caffeine result in improved sleep?). Overall, it seems that simple extrapolation may not be appropriate, as effects are far more robust for experimental sleep disruption studies than for intervention studies designed to improve sleep. This may be due, in part, to the original aims of the investigators because sleep disruption studies were not typically designed to test sleep hygiene recommendations. Future work should continue to evaluate behavioral strategies to improve sleep and address methodological and sample limitations of the extant literature.

Second, current recommendations are somewhat vague and inconsistent, and the evidence is often based on extreme circumstances. For example, the most consistent finding regarding caffeine and sleep is that large doses of caffeine just before bedtime have a negative impact on sleep. However, this evidence may not apply to individuals who do not consume large amounts of caffeine near bedtime. Similarly, with regard to smoking and alcohol use, the effects of light and occasional use are far less clear than the effects of dependent use. Though both acute exercise and habitual exercise training seem to confer modest improvements in sleep, the extant literature does not provide conclusive evidence regarding how to optimize the timing, mode, and dose of exercise to enhance sleep. This does not imply that these recommendations cannot be useful for the general population, but processes should be developed to assist individuals with evaluating their specific circumstances and identifying those behaviors most likely to result in sleep disruption. Across individual recommendations, additional research to inform more specific guidelines and cover a broader spectrum of sleep hygiene behavior would be beneficial.

Finally, certain recommendations would benefit from additional guidance regarding implementation of change. For example, noise reduction and stress management techniques are encouraged, but little guidance is available regarding techniques to use in particular circumstances; consequently, it is up to individuals to identify and employ appropriate strategies. Overall, the limited support for individual sleep hygiene recommendations in the general adult population is not the result of null effects, but rather the substantial need for replication and extension of current work. Sleep hygiene education has the potential to be a key strategy for improving sleep in the general population, and future research has the potential to extend its utility and evaluate its effectiveness.

In order to maximize the utility of sleep hygiene education in the general population, future research should address several key issues. First, the field would benefit from studies of sleep hygiene recommendations that are applicable to natural behavior patterns. Most sleep hygiene recommendations draw upon research that was not intended to test the validity of behavioral recommendations to improve sleep. Thus, much of the empirical foundation for sleep hygiene recommendations in the general population has tested artificial, and often extreme, behaviors in laboratory settings. Laboratory studies have been effective in demonstrating basic effects, but this work should be expanded to examine sleep hygiene behaviors in their naturalistic context. In particular, the impact of habituation should be carefully considered, as it may inform modification of existing recommendations. For example, tolerance to the sleep-disrupting effects of acute caffeine use develops within days, so it is likely more informative to consider an individual's deviation from regular use, as opposed to their habitual daily intake. Regular use of alcohol and nicotine also results in tolerance and dependence, which may moderate their effects on sleep. Thus, different recommendations that take into account the sleep-disrupting effects of substance withdrawal might be needed for dependent and nondependent users of these substances. Future research investigating the impact of naturalistic behavior patterns on sleep will help develop sleep hygiene recommendations that are applicable and appropriate for a broader audience.

The complex interplay between behavioral and environmental patterns must also be considered in the context of sleep hygiene. Modification of one sleep hygiene behavior may lead to unintended (and sometimes undesirable) changes in other behaviors. For example, caffeine withdrawal has been associated with increased stress and decreased exercise, 131 which could result in a counterproductive adverse impact on sleep. Similarly, a reduction in napping may lead to increased caffeine use to combat daytime sleepiness. 124 In contrast, some combinations of sleep hygiene components may result in more collective sleep improvement. For example, daily exercise has been shown to decrease sleep disturbance during smoking cessation. 132 Thus, it is important for future research to evaluate these interrelationships among sleep hygiene components as they relate to sleep disturbance. Individuals attempting to improve their sleep with sleep hygiene recommendations should be made aware of the potential benefits and consequences of modifying multiple aspects of their behavior and, ultimately, attempts should be made to develop guidelines to assist individuals in navigating the complex process of multiple behavior change. A comprehensive lifestyle approach to sleep hygiene education would also be enhanced by further investigation into other behavioral and environmental factors known to impact sleep, such as use of nighttime television and electronic devices, interpersonal environment, and use of over-the-counter sleep aids (e.g., melatonin).

In addition, as noted in the present review and others, 3 , 11 , 23 , 37 , 47 , 124 increased attention must be dedicated to understanding individual differences in the use and effectiveness of sleep hygiene recommendations. Many factors may affect the prevalence, nature, and impact of sleep-disrupting behaviors (e.g., age, gender, genetic polymorphisms, education, comorbid health conditions, social or occupational demands). In an early, personalized approach to sleep hygiene education, Hauri proposed one-time consultations to provide 2-4 personalized sleep hygiene recommendations to adults with insomnia. 76 The majority of patients reported that they tried the personalized recommendations and, with the exception of eliminating alarm clocks, over two-thirds of patients felt the recommendations helped them sleep at one-month follow-up, and many of these perceived benefits were retained over one year. Others have implemented individualized sleep hygiene recommendations in medical patient populations and have reported improvements in subjective sleep quality, 17 , 133 but not actigraphy-assessed sleep. 17 Identification of reliable effect modifiers in future research will be critical to identifying in whom and under what circumstances specific sleep hygiene recommendations will be most effective and will allow for the development of a personalized form of sleep hygiene education appropriate for the general population.

Finally, it is important to note that poor sleep hygiene practices may be a compensatory response to sleep problems or disorders (e.g., self-medicating with alcohol, increased use of caffeine for daytime sleepiness). If these behaviors exist solely as a direct result of the sleep problems, modification of these behaviors is not likely to resolve the underlying sleep problem. The bidirectional relationship between waking behavior and sleep is quite complex with each positioned to influence the other. Thus, it can be difficult to identify the most appropriate target for intervention. In a recent study, Irish and colleagues examined the bidirectional influence of actigraphy-assessed sleep characteristics and waking health behaviors in midlife women. Results indicated that weekly patterns of caffeine use, alcohol use, and smoking predicted some subsequent sleep characteristics, but that sleep did not predict subsequent behaviors. 134 This may suggest that targeting waking behaviors (e.g., sleep hygiene education) has more potential to impact sleep than vice versa, but conclusions are limited by the low prevalence of waking behaviors in this sample. 134 Ultimately, modification of behaviors is a reasonable approach to sleep improvement efforts, but it is important to remember that sleep will, in turn, exert influence over subsequent waking behaviors and that some sleep problems (e.g., severe insomnia, obstructive sleep apnea) will require additional forms of treatment.

In conclusion, future research should aim to substantiate and improve the efficacy of specific sleep hygiene recommendations in the general population. This is especially important given its cost-effectiveness, ease of dissemination, and accessibility. These questions are critical to public health given current trends in sleep patterns, including subclinical sleep complaints in the general population. 135 Sleep hygiene education may find a new purpose in the promotion of sleep and population health.

Practice Points.

Sleep hygiene education has the potential to address the growing public health concern of sleep complaints in the general population.

Although each recommendation is theoretically sound and plausible, a review of individual sleep hygiene recommendations regarding caffeine use, smoking, alcohol use, exercise, stress, noise, sleep timing, and napping revealed that empirical support for these recommendations in the general population is lacking.

Much of the current knowledge regarding individual components of sleep hygiene in nonclinical samples is limited to acute effects tested in laboratory environments.

Research Agenda.

Replication and extension of research demonstrating the effects of individual sleep hygiene components on objectively-assessed sleep is required, particularly in more naturalistic contexts.

Improved understanding of the role of habituation to sleep hygiene behaviors and their impact on sleep is an important next step toward establishing practical recommendations.

The complex interplay amongst sleep hygiene behaviors should be evaluated to inform development of effective, personalized strategies to maximize sleep improvement.

Additional consideration of behavioral and environmental factors relevant to the general population is warranted (e.g., use of television and other electronics, use of over-the-counter sleep aids, social environment).

Future research investigating the impact of individual differences on the use and relevance of individual sleep hygiene components will inform future efforts to develop effective personalized sleep hygiene interventions appropriate for nonclinical populations.

Acknowledgements

Grant support for Dr. Irish was provided by NIH MH019986, and support for Drs. Kline and Gunn was provided by NIH HL082610. Dr. Buysse received support from NIH MH024642 and AG020677 and Dr. Hall received support from NIH HL104607.

Dr. Buysse has served as a paid or unpaid consultant on scientific advisory boards for the following companies: Merck, Philips Respironics, Purdue Pharma and General Sleep Corporation. Dr. Buysse has also spoken at single-sponsored educational meetings for Servier. He has also spoken at a single-sponsored lecture for Astellas. Total fees from each of these sources was less than $10,000 per year.

Abbreviations

Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Insomnia

electroencephalography

gamma-aminobutyric acid

intensive care unit

non rapid eye movement

polysomnography

rapid eye movement

sleep efficiency

sleep onset latency

slow wave sleep

total sleep time

wake after sleep onset

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

  • 1. Hauri P. Sleep hygiene. In: Hauri P, editor. Current Concepts: The Sleep Disorders. The Upjohn Company; Kalamazoo, MI: 1977. pp. 21–35. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 2. Zarcone VP. Sleep hygiene. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 3rd ed. WB Saunders; Philadelphia, PA: 2000. pp. 657–61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 3*. Stepanski EJ, Wyatt JK. Use of sleep hygiene in the treatment of insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7:215–25. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0246. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 4. Friedman L, Zeitzer JM, Mumenthaler MS. Role of healthy sleep practices: Alcohol/caffeine/exercise/scheduling. In: Sateia MJ, Buysse DJ, editors. Insomnia: Diagnosis and Treatment. Informa; London: 2010. pp. 260–7. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 5. Riedel BW. Sleep hygiene. In: Lichstein KL, Morin CM, editors. Treatment of late-life insomnia. Sage; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2000. pp. 125–46. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 6. Morgenthaler T, Kramer M, Alessi C, Friedman L, Boehlecke B, Brown T, et al. Practice parameters for the psychological and behavioral treatment of insomnia: an update. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine report. Sleep. 2006;29:1415–9. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 7. Leger D, Poursain B, Neubauer D, Uchiyama M. An international survey of sleeping problems in the general population. Curr Med Res Opin. 2008;24:307–17. doi: 10.1185/030079907x253771. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 8. Soldatos CR, Allaert FA, Ohta T, Dikeos DG. How do individuals sleep around the world? Results from a single-day survey in ten countries. Sleep Med. 2005;6:5–13. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2004.10.006. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 9. Banks S, Dinges DF. Behavioral and physiological consequences of sleep restriction. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:519–28. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 10. Walker MP. The role of sleep in cognition and emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:168–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04416.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 11*. Zaharna M, Guilleminault C. Sleep, noise and health: review. Noise Health. 2010;12:64–9. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.63205. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 12. Buysse DJ. Sleep health: can we define it? Does it matter? Sleep. 2014;37:9–17. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3298. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 13. Morin CM. Chronic insomnia: recent advances and innovations in treatment development and dissemination. Can Psychol. 2010;51:31–9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 14. Berger AM, Kuhn BR, Farr LA, Lynch JC, Agrawal S, Chamberlain J, et al. Behavioral therapy intervention trial to improve sleep quality and cancer-related fatigue. Psychooncology. 2009;18:634–46. doi: 10.1002/pon.1438. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 15. Berger AM, VonEssen S, Khun BR, Piper BF, Farr L, Agrawal S, et al. Feasibilty of a sleep intervention during adjuvant breast cancer chemotherapy. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2002;29:1431–41. doi: 10.1188/02.ONF.1431-1441. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 16. Chen PH, Kuo HY, Chueh KH. Sleep hygiene education: efficacy on sleep quality in working women. J Nurs Res. 2010;18:283–9. doi: 10.1097/JNR.0b013e3181fbe3fd. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 17. Hudson AL, Portillo CJ, Lee KA. Sleep disturbances in women with HIV or AIDS: efficacy of a tailored sleep promotion intervention. Nurs Res. 2008;57:360–6. doi: 10.1097/01.NNR.0000313501.84604.2c. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 18. Kakinuma M, Takahashi M, Kato N, Aratake Y, Watanabe M, Ishikawa Y, et al. Effect of brief sleep hygiene education for workers of an information technology company. Ind Health. 2010;48:758–65. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.ms1083. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 19. Morita E, Miyazaki S, Okawa M. Pilot study on the effects of a 1-day sleep education program: influence on sleep of stopping alcohol intake at bedtime. Nagoya J Med Sci. 2012;74:359–65. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 20. Yngman-Uhlin P, Fernstrom A, Borjeson S, Edell-Gustafsson U. Evaluation of an individual sleep intervention programme in people undergoing peritoneal dialysis treatment. J Clin Nurs. 2012;21:3402–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2012.04282.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 21. Nehlig A. Are we dependent upon coffee and caffeine? A review on human and animal data. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:563–76. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(98)00050-5. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 22. Boutrel B, Koob GF. What keeps us awake: the neuropharmacology of stimulants and wakefulness-promoting medications. Sleep. 2004;27:1181–94. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.6.1181. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 23*. Roehrs T, Roth T. Caffeine: sleep and daytime sleepiness. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12:153–62. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.004. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 24. Polasek TM, Patel F, Jensen BP, Sorich MJ, Wiese MD, Doogue MP. Predicted metabolic drug clearance with increasing adult age. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2013;75:1019–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2012.04446.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 25. Landolt HP, Werth E, Borbely AA, Dijk DJ. Caffeine intake (200 mg) in the morning affects human sleep and EEG power spectra at night. Brain Res. 1995;675:67–74. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00040-w. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 26. Youngberg MR, Karpov IO, Begley A, Pollock BG, Buysse DJ. Clinical and physiological correlates of caffeine and caffeine metabolites in primary insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7:196–203. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 27. Drake C, Roehrs T, Shambroom J, Roth T. Caffeine effects on sleep taken 0, 3, or 6 hours before going to bed. J Clin Sleep Med. 2013;9:1195–200. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.3170. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 28. Bunker ML, McWilliams M. Caffeine content of common beverages. J Am Diet Assoc. 1979;74:28–32. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 29. Nicholson AN, Stone BM. Heterocyclic amphetamine derivatives and caffeine on sleep in man. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1980;9:195–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.1980.tb05833.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 30. Lloret-Linares C, Lafuente-Lafuente C, Chassany O, Green A, Delcey Y, Mouly S, et al. Does a single cup of coffee at dinner alter the sleep? A controlled cross-over randomised trial in real-life conditions. Nutr Diet. 2012;69:250–5. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 31. Rétey JV, Adam M, Khatami R, Luhmann UFO, Jung HH, Berger W, et al. A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2007;81:692–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.clpt.6100102. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 32. Keenan EK, Tiplady B, Priestley CM, Rogers PJ. Naturalistic effects of five days of bedtime caffeine use on sleep, next-day cognitive performance, and mood. J Caff Res. 2014;4:13–20. doi: 10.1089/jcr.2011.0030. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 33. Hindmarch I, Rigney U, Stanley N, Quinlan P, Rycroft J, Lane J. A naturalistic investigation of the effects of day-long consumption of tea, coffee and water on alertness, sleep onset and sleep quality. Psychopharmacol. 2000;149:203–16. doi: 10.1007/s002130000383. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 34. Ho SC, Chung JW. The effects of caffeine abstinence on sleep: a pilot study. Appl Nurs Res. 2013;26:80–4. doi: 10.1016/j.apnr.2012.08.004. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 35. Hofer I, Battig K. Cardiovascular, behavioral, and subjective effects of caffeine under field conditions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;48:899–908. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90198-8. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 36. James JE. Acute and chronic effects of caffeine on performance, mood, headache, and sleep. Neuropsychobiology. 1998;38:32–41. doi: 10.1159/000026514. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 37*. Jaehne A, Loessl B, Barkai Z, Riemann D, Hornyak M. Effects of nicotine on sleep during consumption, withdrawal and replacement therapy. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13:363–77. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2008.12.003. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 38. Hughes JR. Effects of abstinence from tobacco: valid symptoms and time course. Nicotine Tob Res. 2007;9:315–27. doi: 10.1080/14622200701188919. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 39. Colrain IM, Trinder J, Swan GE. The impact of smoking cessation on objective and subjective markers of sleep: review, synthesis, and recommendations. Nicotine Tob Res. 2004;6:913–25. doi: 10.1080/14622200412331324938. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 40. Wetter DW, Young TB. The relation between cigarette smoking and sleep disturbance. Prev Med. 1994;23:328–34. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1994.1046. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 41. Zhang L, Samet J, Caffo B, Punjabi NM. Cigarette smoking and nocturnal sleep architecture. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;164:529–37. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj231. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 42. Jaehne A, Unbehaun T, Feige B, Lutz UC, Batra A, Riemann D. How smoking affects sleep: a polysomnographical analysis. Sleep Med. 2012;13:1286–92. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.06.026. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 43. Gillin JC, Lardon M, Ruiz C, Golshan S, Salin-Pascual R. Dose-dependent effects of transdermal nicotine on early morning awakening and rapid eye movement sleep time in nonsmoking normal volunteers. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1994;14:264–7. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 44. Nakata A, Takahashi M, Haratani T, Ikeda T, Hojou M, Fujioka Y, et al. Association of active and passive smoking with sleep disturbances and short sleep duration among japanese working population. Int J Behav Med. 2008;15:81–91. doi: 10.1080/10705500801929577. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 45. Sabanayagam C, Shankar A. The association between active smoking, smokeless tobacco, second-hand smoke exposure and insufficient sleep. Sleep Med. 2011;12:7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.09.002. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 46. Davila EP, Lee DJ, Fleming LE, LeBlanc WG, Arheart K, Dietz N, et al. Sleep disorders and secondhand smoke exposure in the U.S. population. Nicotine Tob Res. 2010;12:294–9. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntp193. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 47*. Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness, and alcohol use. Alcohol Res Health. 2001;25:101–9. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 48. Stein MD, Friedmann PD. Disturbed sleep and its relationship to alcohol use. Subst Abus. 2005;26:1–13. doi: 10.1300/j465v26n01_01. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 49*. Ebrahim IO, Shapiro CM, Williams AJ, Fenwick PB. Alcohol and sleep I: effects on normal sleep. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37:539–49. doi: 10.1111/acer.12006. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 50. Thakkar MM, Engemann SC, Sharma R, Sahota P. Role of wake-promoting basal forebrain and adenosinergic mechanisms in sleep-promoting effects of ethanol. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:997–1005. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01174.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 51. Williams HL, Rundell OH., Jr Altered sleep physiology in chronic alcoholics: reversal with abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1981;5:318–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1981.tb04905.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 52. Drummond SP, Gillin JC, Smith TL, DeModena A. The sleep of abstinent pure primary alcoholic patients: natural course and relationship to relapse. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1998;22:1796–802. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 53. Landolt HP, Gillin JC. Sleep abnormalities during abstinence in alcohol-dependent patients. Aetiology and management. CNS Drugs. 2001;15:413–25. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200115050-00006. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 54. Rehm J, Room R, Graham K, Monteiro M, Gmel G, Sempos CT. The relationship of average volume of alcohol consumption and patterns of drinking to burden of disease: an overview. Addiction. 2003;98:1209–28. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00467.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 55. Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness, sleep disorders and alcohol use and abuse. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5:287–97. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0162. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 56. Landolt HP, Roth C, Dijk DJ, Borbely AA. Late-afternoon ethanol intake affects nocturnal sleep and the sleep EEG in middle-aged men. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1996;16:428–36. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199612000-00004. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 57. Yules RB, Lippman ME, Freedman DX. Alcohol administration prior to sleep. The effect on EEG sleep stages. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1967;16:94–7. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1967.01730190096012. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 58. Graham K, Wilsnack R, Dawson D, Vogeltanz N. Should alcohol consumption measures be adjusted for gender differences? Addiction. 1998;93:1137–47. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.93811372.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 59. Istvan J, Matarazzo JD. Tobacco, alcohol, and caffeine use: a review of their interrelationships. Psychol Bull. 1984;95:301–26. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 60. Youngstedt SD. Effects of exercise on sleep. Clin Sports Med. 2005;24:355–65. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2004.12.003. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 61. Kubitz KA, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M. The effects of acute and chronic exercise on sleep. A meta-analytic review. Sports Med. 1996;21:277–91. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199621040-00004. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 62. Youngstedt SD, O'Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effects of acute exercise on sleep: a quantitative synthesis. Sleep. 1997;20:203–14. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.3.203. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 63. Passos GS, Poyares D, Santana MG, Garbuio SA, Tufik S, de Mello MT. Effect of acute physical exercise on patients with chronic primary insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2010;6:270–5. [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 64*. Yang PY, Ho KH, Chen HC, Chien MY. Exercise training improves sleep quality in middle-aged and older adults with sleep problems: a systematic review. J Physiother. 2012;58:157–63. doi: 10.1016/S1836-9553(12)70106-6. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 65. Singh NA, Stavrinos TM, Scarbek Y, Galambos G, Liber C, Fiatarone Singh MA. A randomized controlled trial of high versus low intensity weight training versus general practitioner care for clinical depression in older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2005;60:768–76. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.6.768. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 66. Kline CE, Sui X, Hall MH, Youngstedt SD, Blair SN, Earnest CP, et al. Dose-response effects of exercise training on the subjective sleep quality of postmenopausal women: exploratory analyses of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open. 2012;2:e001044. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001044. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 67. Krauchi K, Cajochen C, Werth E, Wirz-Justice A. Functional link between distal vasodilation and sleep-onset latency? Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2000;278:R741–8. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.278.3.R741. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 68. Horne JA, Moore VJ. Sleep EEG effects of exercise with and without additional body cooling. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;60:33–8. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)90948-4. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 69. Myllymaki T, Kyrolainen H, Savolainen K, Hokka L, Jakonen R, Juuti T, et al. Effects of vigorous late-night exercise on sleep quality and cardiac autonomic activity. J Sleep Res. 2011;20:146–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00874.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 70. Flausino NH, Da Silva Prado JM, de Queiroz SS, Tufik S, de Mello MT. Physical exercise performed before bedtime improves the sleep pattern of healthy young good sleepers. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:186–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01300.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 71. Buman MP, Phillips B, Youngstedt SD, Kline CE, Hirshkowitz M. Does nighttime exercise really disturb sleep? Results from the 2013 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Sleep Med. 2014;15:755–61. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.01.008. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 72. Dzierzewski JM, Buman MP, Giacobbi PR, Jr, Roberts BL, Aiken-Morgan AT, Marsiske M, et al. Exercise and sleep in community-dwelling older adults: evidence for a reciprocal relationship. J Sleep Res. 2014;23:61–8. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12078. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 73*. Kim EJ, Dimsdale JE. The effect of psychosocial stress on sleep: a review of polysomnographic evidence. Behav Sleep Med. 2007;5:256–78. doi: 10.1080/15402000701557383. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 74. Morin CM, Rodrigue S, Ivers H. Role of stress, arousal, and coping skills in primary insomnia. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:259–67. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000030391.09558.a3. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 75. Hall M, Vasko R, Buysse D, Ombao H, Chen Q, Cashmere JD, et al. Acute stress affects heart rate variability during sleep. Psychosom Med. 2004;66:56–62. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000106884.58744.09. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 76. Hauri PJ. Consulting about insomnia: a method and some preliminary data. Sleep. 1993;16:344–50. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.4.344. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 77. Hauri PJ. Case Studies in Insomnia. Plenum; New York, NY: 1991. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 78. Means MK, Lichstein KL, Epperson MT, Johnson CT. Relaxation therapy for insomnia: nighttime and day time effects. Behav Res Ther. 2000;38:665–78. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(99)00091-1. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 79. Lichstein KL, Riedel BW, Wilson NM, Lester KW, Aguillard RN. Relaxation and sleep compression for late-life insomnia: a placebo-controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2001;69:227–39. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.2.227. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 80. Borkovec TD, Fowles DC. Controlled investigation of the effects of progressive and hypnotic relaxation on insomnia. J Abnorm Psychol. 1973;82:153–8. doi: 10.1037/h0034970. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 81. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2003;10:144–56. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 82. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits. A meta-analysis. J Psychosom Res. 2004;57:35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 83. Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Naranjo JR, Schmidt S. Influence of mindfulness practice on cortisol and sleep in long-term and short-term meditators. Neuropsychobiology. 2012;65:109–18. doi: 10.1159/000330362. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 84. Caldwell K, Emery L, Harrison M, Greeson J. Changes in mindfulness, well-being, and sleep quality in college students through taijiquan courses: a cohort control study. J Altern Complement Med. 2011;17:931–8. doi: 10.1089/acm.2010.0645. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 85. Ong JC, Shapiro SL, Manber R. Combining mindfulness meditation with cognitive-behavior therapy for insomnia: a treatment-development study. Behav Ther. 2008;39:171–82. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.07.002. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 86. Van Reeth O, Weibel L, Spiegel K, Leproult R, Dugovic C, Maccari S. Interactions between stress and sleep: from basic research to clinical situations. Sleep Med Rev. 2000;4:201–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 87. Cartwright RD, Wood E. Adjustment disorders of sleep: the sleep effects of a major stressful event and its resolution. Psychiatry Res. 1991;39:199–209. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90088-7. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 88. Sadeh A, Keinan G, Daon K. Effects of stress on sleep: the moderating role of coping style. Health Psychol. 2004;23:542–5. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.5.542. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 89. Petersen H, Kecklund G, D'Onofrio P, Nilsson J, Akerstedt T. Stress vulnerability and the effects of moderate daily stress on sleep polysomnography and subjective sleepiness. J Sleep Res. 2013;22:50–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2012.01034.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 90. Muzet A. Environmental noise, sleep and health. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:135–42. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2006.09.001. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 91. Griefahn B, Brode P, Marks A, Basner M. Autonomic arousals related to traffic noise during sleep. Sleep. 2008;31:569–77. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.4.569. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 92. Kawada T, Xin P, Kuroiwa M, Sasazawa Y, Suzuki S, Tamura Y. Habituation of sleep to road traffic noise as determined by polysomnography and an accelerometer. J Sound Vib. 2001;242:169–78. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 93. Xie H, Kang J, Mills GH. Clinical review: the impact of noise on patients' sleep and the effectiveness of noise reduction strategies in intensive care units. Crit Care. 2009;13:208. doi: 10.1186/cc7154. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 94. Eberhardt JL, Akselsson KR. The disturbance by road traffic noise of the sleep of young male adults as recorded in the home. J Sound Vib. 1987;114:417–34. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 95. Amundsen AH, Klaeboe R, Aasvang GM. Long-term effects of noise reduction measures on noise annoyance and sleep disturbance: the Norwegian facade insulation study. J Acoust Soc Am. 2013;133:3921–8. doi: 10.1121/1.4802824. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 96. Wilkinson RT, Campbell KB. Effects of traffic noise on quality of sleep: assessment by EEG, subjective report, or performance the next day. J Acoust Soc Am. 1984;75:468–75. doi: 10.1121/1.390470. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 97. Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J Neurosci. 1995;15:3526–38. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-03526.1995. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 98. Dijk DJ, Lockley SW. Integration of human sleep-wake regulation and circadian rhythmicity. J Appl Physiol. 2002;92:852–62. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2001. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 99. Jefferson CD, Drake CL, Scofield HM, Myers E, McClure T, Roehrs T, et al. Sleep hygiene practices in a population-based sample of insomniacs. Sleep. 2005;28:611–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.5.611. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 100. Buysse DJ, Cheng Y, Germain A, Moul DE, Franzen PL, Fletcher M, et al. Night-to-night sleep variability in older adults with and without chronic insomnia. Sleep Med. 2010;11:56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.02.010. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 101. Morin CM. Psychological and behavioral treatments for insomnia I: approaches and efficacy. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 5th ed. Elsevier Saunders; St. Louis, MO: 2011. pp. 866–83. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 102. Gellis LA, Lichstein KL. Sleep hygiene practices of good and poor sleepers in the United States: an internet-based study. Behav Ther. 2009;40:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2008.02.001. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 103. Billiard M, Alperovitch A, Perot C, Jammes A. Excessive daytime somnolence in young men: prevalence and contributing factors. Sleep. 1987;10:297–305. doi: 10.1093/sleep/10.4.297. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 104. Carney CE, Edinger JD, Meyer B, Lindman L, Istre T. Daily activities and sleep quality in college students. Chronobiol Int. 2006;23:623–37. doi: 10.1080/07420520600650695. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 105. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Billy BD, Fletcher ME, Kennedy KS, Schlarb JE, et al. Circadian type and bed-timing regularity in 654 retired seniors: correlations with subjective sleep measures. Sleep. 2011;34:235–9. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.2.235. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 106. Soehner AM, Kennedy KS, Monk TH. Circadian preference and sleep-wake regularity: associations with self-report sleep parameters in daytime-working adults. Chronobiol Int. 2011;28:802–9. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.613137. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 107. Monk TH, Reynolds CF III, Buysse DJ, DeGrazia JM, Kupfer DJ. The relationship between lifestyle regularity and subjective sleep quality. Chronobiol Int. 2003;20:97–107. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120017812. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 108. Bonnet MH, Alter J. Effects of irregular versus regular sleep schedules on performance, mood and body temperature. Biol Psychol. 1982;14:287–96. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(82)90009-6. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 109. Takasu NN, Takenaka Y, Fujiwara M, Toichi M. Effects of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime autonomic functions and psychological states in healthy university students with irregular sleep-wake habits. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2012;10:84–93. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 110. Youngstedt SD, Kline CE, Zielinski MR, Kripke DF, Devlin TM, Bogan RK, et al. Tolerance of chronic 90-minute time-in-bed restriction in older long sleepers. Sleep. 2009;32:1467–79. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1467. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 111. Manber R, Bootzin RR, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. The effects of regularizing sleep-wake schedules on daytime sleepiness. Sleep. 1996;19:432–41. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.5.432. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 112. Buysse DJ, Browman KE, Monk TH, Reynolds CF III, Fasiczka AL, Kupfer DJ. Napping and 24-hour sleep/wake patterns in healthy elderly and young adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40:779–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01849.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 113. Goldman SE, Hall M, Boudreau R, Matthews KA, Cauley JA, Ancoli-Israel S, et al. Association between nighttime sleep and napping in older adults. Sleep. 2008;31:733–40. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.5.733. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 114. McDevitt EA, Alaynick WA, Mednick SC. The effect of nap frequency on daytime sleep architecture. Physiol Behav. 2012;107:40–4. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.021. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 115. Jean-Louis G, Kripke DF, Assmus JD, Langer RD. Sleep-wake patterns among postmenopausal women: a 24-hour unattended polysomnographic study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2000;55:M120–3. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.3.m120. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 116. Pilcher JJ, Michalowski KR, Carrigan RD. The prevalence of daytime napping and its relationship to nighttime sleep. Behav Med. 2001;27:71–6. doi: 10.1080/08964280109595773. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 117. Foley DJ, Vitiello MV, Bliwise DL, Ancoli-Israel S, Monjan AA, Walsh JK. Frequent napping is associated with excessive daytime sleepiness, depression, pain, and nocturia in older adults: findings from the National Sleep Foundation ‘2003 Sleep in America’ Poll. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15:344–50. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000249385.50101.67. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 118. Campbell SS, Stanchina MD, Schlang JR, Murphy PJ. Effects of a month-long napping regimen in older individuals. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59:224–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.03264.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 119. Aber R, Webb WB. Effects of a limited nap on night sleep in older subjects. Psychol Aging. 1986;1:300–2. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.1.4.300. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 120. Feinberg I, March JD, Floyd TC, Jimison R, Bossom-Demitrack L, Katz PH. Homeostatic changes during post-nap sleep maintain baseline levels of delta EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;61:134–7. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)91051-x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 121. Campbell SS, Murphy PJ, Stauble TN. Effects of a nap on nighttime sleep and waking function in older subjects. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:48–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53009.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 122. Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Carrier J, Billy BD, Rose LR. Effects of afternoon “siesta” naps on sleep, alertness, performance, and circadian rhythms in the elderly. Sleep. 2001;24:680–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/24.6.680. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 123. Werth E, Dijk DJ, Achermann P, Borbely AA. Dynamics of the sleep EEG after an early evening nap: experimental data and simulations. Am J Physiol. 1996;271:R501–10. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.271.3.R501. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 124*. Dhand R, Sohal H. Good sleep, bad sleep! The role of daytime naps in healthy adults. Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2006;12:379–82. doi: 10.1097/01.mcp.0000245703.92311.d0. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 125*. Takahashi M. The role of prescribed napping in sleep medicine. Sleep Med Rev. 2003;7:227–35. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0241. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 126. Pilcher JJ, Michalowski KR, Carrigan RD. The prevalence of daytime napping and its relationship to nighttime sleep. Behav Med. 2001;27:71–6. doi: 10.1080/08964280109595773. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 127. Yoon IY, Kripke DF, Elliott JA, Langer RD. Naps and circadian rhythms in postmenopausal women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59:844–8. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.8.m844. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 128. Dautovich ND, McCrae CS, Rowe M. Subjective and objective napping and sleep in older adults: are evening naps “bad” for nighttime sleep? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1681–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01822.x. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 129. Monk TH. Aging human circadian rhythms: conventional wisdom may not always be right. J Biol Rhythms. 2005;20:366–74. doi: 10.1177/0748730405277378. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 130. Yoon IY, Kripke DF, Youngstedt SD, Elliott JA. Actigraphy suggests age-related differences in napping and nocturnal sleep. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:87–93. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00345.x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 131. Juliano LM, Griffiths RR. A critical review of caffeine withdrawal: empirical validation of symptoms and signs, incidence, severity, and associated features. Psychopharmacology. 2004;176:1–29. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2000-x. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 132. Grove JR, Wilkinson A, Dawson B, Eastwood P, Heard P. Effects of exercise on subjective aspects of sleep during tobacco withdrawal. Aust Psychol. 2006;41:69–76. [ Google Scholar ]
  • 133. Suzuki E, Tsuchiya M, Hirokawa K, Taniguchi T, Mitsuhashi T, Kawakami N. Evaluation of an internet-based self-help program for better quality of sleep among Japanese workers: a randomized controlled trial. J Occup Health. 2008;50:387–99. doi: 10.1539/joh.l7154. [ DOI ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 134. Irish LA, Kline CE, Rothenberger SD, Krafty RT, Buysse DJ, Kravitz HM, et al. A 24-hour approach to the study of health behaviors: Temporal relationships between waking health behaviors and sleep. Ann Behav Med. 2014;47:189–97. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9533-3. [ DOI ] [ PMC free article ] [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • 135. Institute of Medicine . Sleep Disorders and Sleep Deprivation: An Unmet Public Health Problem. The National Academies Press; Washington, DC: 2006. [ PubMed ] [ Google Scholar ]
  • View on publisher site
  • PDF (222.1 KB)
  • Collections

Similar articles

Cited by other articles, links to ncbi databases.

  • Download .nbib .nbib
  • Format: AMA APA MLA NLM

Add to Collections

IMAGES

  1. (PDF) Sleeping Habits and Perception of Its Health Effects among

    research paper about sleeping habits

  2. (PDF) Sleeping Habits, Classroom Behaviour and Academic Performance of

    research paper about sleeping habits

  3. The Art of Sleep: Cultivating Healthy Sleeping Habits Free Essay Example

    research paper about sleeping habits

  4. How To Improve My Sleeping Habits

    research paper about sleeping habits

  5. (PDF) Sleep Habits and Patterns of College Students: A Preliminary Study

    research paper about sleeping habits

  6. (PDF) Sleep habits as predictors of psychological health in healthcare

    research paper about sleeping habits

VIDEO

  1. the planets and their sleeping habits #planethumans #oc #planets

  2. *BENEFITS OF GOOD SLEEPING HABITS #sleepwell

  3. Sleeping Position Can Save Your Life #shorts

  4. Sleeping habits #shortstamil #shorts

  5. Planning Contents And Reviewing Sleeping Habits 🤣🤣🤣💕🐶🐶🐶🐶

  6. All 5 MY SWWI Fanmades Song Combined